Spatial Distortions Produced by Purely Dichoptic-Based Visual Motion

It is known that local, monocular motion (short-range motion) can produce local distortions of visual space. We wanted to know if local monocular motion was both sufficient and necessary for producing motion-based spatial distortions. We used a previously reported dichoptic motion stimulus in which the directional motion signal is not present in either eye's input but is only present after binocular combination. We show that such a stimulus can also induce perceived changes in spatial position. This suggests that local, monocular motion while being sufficient is not necessary for the production of motion-based illusions. It suggests that one source of motion signals responsible for this illusion is from binocular motion mechanisms.

[1]  Katsumi Watanabe,et al.  The motion-induced position shift depends on the visual awareness of motion , 2005, Vision Research.

[2]  Mark A. Georgeson,et al.  No evidence for dichoptic motion sensing: a reply to carney and shadlen , 1992, Vision Research.

[3]  Johan Wagemans,et al.  Texture and object motion in slant discrimination: failure of reliability-based weighting of cues may be evidence for strong fusion. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[4]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Motion: the long and short of it. , 1989, Spatial vision.

[5]  Y. Dan,et al.  Asymmetry in Visual Cortical Circuits Underlying Motion-Induced Perceptual Mislocalization , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  David Whitaker,et al.  Non-veridical size perception of expanding and contracting objects , 1999, Vision Research.

[7]  Paul V McGraw,et al.  Motion-Sensitive Neurones in V5/MT Modulate Perceived Spatial Position , 2004, Current Biology.

[8]  Mark A. Georgeson,et al.  Monocular motion sensing, binocular motion perception , 1989, Vision Research.

[9]  O. Braddick A short-range process in apparent motion. , 1974, Vision research.

[10]  K. D. De Valois,et al.  Vernier acuity with stationary moving Gabors. , 1991, Vision research.

[11]  David Whitney,et al.  Flexible retinotopy: motion-dependent position coding in the visual cortex. , 2010, Science.

[12]  F A Wichmann,et al.  Ning for Helpful Comments and Suggestions. This Paper Benefited Con- Siderably from Conscientious Peer Review, and We Thank Our Reviewers the Psychometric Function: I. Fitting, Sampling, and Goodness of Fit , 2001 .

[13]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  Influence of motion signals on the perceived position of spatial pattern , 1999, Nature.

[14]  David Whitaker,et al.  Motion Adaptation Distorts Perceived Visual Position , 2002, Current Biology.

[15]  D. Bressler,et al.  Spatially asymmetric response to moving patterns in the visual cortex: Re-examining the local sign hypothesis , 2007, Vision Research.

[16]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Mechanisms of human motion perception revealed by a new cyclopean illusion. , 1986, Science.

[17]  Michael N. Shadlen,et al.  Dichoptic activation of the early motion system , 1993, Vision Research.

[18]  R. Snowden,et al.  Shifts in perceived position following adaptation to visual motion , 1998, Current Biology.

[19]  N. Qian,et al.  A Physiological Model for Motion-Stereo Integration and a Unified Explanation of Pulfrich-like Phenomena , 1997, Vision Research.

[20]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  A method for generating a "purely first-order" dichoptic motion stimulus. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[21]  Felix Wichmann,et al.  The psychometric function: II. Bootstrap-based confidence intervals and sampling , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Illusory Displacement of Equiluminous Kinetic Edges , 1990, Perception.