Impulse-response functions for chromatic and achromatic stimuli

Thresholds were measured for detecting pairs of briefly flashed stimuli displayed successively at variable onset asynchronies. The stimuli were 1 cycle/deg vertical sinusoidal gratings, modulated either in luminance (yellow–black) or in color (red–green). The successive presentations were either of the same contrast (positive) or of opposite contrast (negative), yielding four separate summation curves: positive and negative summation for color and for luminance. Both the positive and the negative curves followed a shorter time course for luminance than for color, implying a faster response at threshold. To calculate impulse response functions from the summation data, we assumed that the neural impulse response from two successive stimuli sum linearly at threshold, that thresholds are determined by probability summation of the combined impulse response over time, and that the impulse response can be described by an exponentially damped frequency-modulated sinusoidal function with four free parameters. The predicted impulse responses for luminance and for color are quite different, being biphasic for luminance and monophasic for color. Fourier transform of these functions yielded estimates of the amplitude and the phase functions of hypothetical visual detectors: the amplitude functions predicted well the contrast sensitivity of counterphased gratings (as a function of temporal frequency) both for luminance and for chromatic stimuli.

[1]  J. Pokorny,et al.  Threshold temporal integration of chromatic stimuli , 1984, Vision Research.

[2]  M. Mayer,et al.  Invariance of the slope of the psychometric function with spatial summation. , 1986, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[3]  W. Weibull A Statistical Distribution Function of Wide Applicability , 1951 .

[4]  E H Adelson,et al.  Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[5]  D. Regan,et al.  Wavelength-modulated light generator. , 1971, Vision research.

[6]  Mitsuo Ikeda,et al.  Temporal Summation of Positive and Negative Flashes in the Visual System , 1965 .

[7]  K. Uchikawa,et al.  Temporal integration of chromatic double pulses for detection of equal-luminance wavelength changes. , 1986, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[8]  J. Roufs Dynamic properties of vision. IV. Thresholds of decremental flashes, incremental flashes and doublets in relation to flicker fusion. , 1974, Vision research.

[9]  H DE LANGE DZN,et al.  Research into the dynamic nature of the human fovea-cortex systems with intermittent and modulated light. I. Attenuation characteristics with white and colored light. , 1958, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[10]  Mitsuo Ikeda,et al.  Temporal impulse response , 1986, Vision Research.

[11]  O E Favreau,et al.  Perceived velocity of moving chromatic gratings. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[12]  J. Robson,et al.  Probability summation and regional variation in contrast sensitivity across the visual field , 1981, Vision Research.

[13]  D. Burr,et al.  Seeing objects in motion , 1986, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[14]  Denis G. Pelli,et al.  On the relation between summation and facilitation , 1987, Vision Research.

[15]  J. J. Wisowaty Estimates for the temporal response characteristics of chromatic pathways. , 1981, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[16]  D. H. Kelly Spatio-temporal frequency characteristics of color-vision mechanisms* , 1974 .

[17]  Stuart Anstis,et al.  The contribution of color to motion in normal and color-deficient observers , 1991, Vision Research.

[18]  V. S. RAMACHANDRAN,et al.  Does colour provide an input to human motion perception? , 1978, Nature.

[19]  J. Pokorny,et al.  Chromatic information processing. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  Sven Treitel,et al.  Addendum to "The Stabilization of Two-Dimensional Recursive Filters via the Discrete Hilbert Transform" , 1973 .

[21]  S. Treitel,et al.  The Stabilization of Two-Dimensional Recursive Filters via the Discrete Hilbert Transform , 1973 .

[22]  Joel Pokorny,et al.  Wavelength effects on simple reaction time , 1977 .

[23]  Joel Pokorny,et al.  Responses of macaque ganglion cells and human observers to compound periodic waveforms , 1993, Vision Research.

[24]  C W Tyler,et al.  Psychophysical derivation of the impulse response through generation of ultrabrief responses: complex inverse estimation without minimum-phase assumptions. , 1992, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[25]  V C Smith,et al.  Temporal modulation sensitivity and pulse-detection thresholds for chromatic and luminance perturbations. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[26]  A. Watson Derivation of the impulse response: comments on the method of Roufs and Blommaert , 1982, Vision Research.

[27]  A. Watson,et al.  Quest: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[29]  D G Pelli,et al.  Uncertainty explains many aspects of visual contrast detection and discrimination. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[30]  J. van Santen,et al.  Elaborated Reichardt detectors. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[31]  A. Watson Probability summation over time , 1979, Vision Research.

[32]  John A. Nelder,et al.  A Simplex Method for Function Minimization , 1965, Comput. J..

[33]  H. D. L. Dzn Research into the dynamic nature of the human fovea-cortex systems with intermittent and modulated light. I. Attenuation characteristics with white and colored light. , 1958 .

[34]  F. J. J. Blommaert,et al.  Temporal impulse and step responses of the human eye obtained psychophysically by means of a drift-correcting perturbation technique , 1981, Vision Research.

[35]  J. Victor Temporal impulse responses from flicker sensitivities: causality, linearity, and amplitude data do not determine phase. , 1989, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[36]  D. Burr,et al.  Summation of Target and Mask Metacontrast Stimuli , 1984, Perception.

[37]  Christopher W. Tyler,et al.  Waveform optimization for phase reconstruction of the impulse response , 1993 .

[38]  J. Roufs,et al.  Dynamic properties of vision. II. Theoretical relationships between flicker and flash thresholds. , 1972, Vision research.

[39]  A. Watson,et al.  Patterns of temporal interaction in the detection of gratings , 1977, Vision Research.

[40]  J. Pokorny,et al.  Metacontrast masking depends on luminance transients , 1977, Vision Research.

[41]  David C. Burr,et al.  Temporal Characteristics of Colour Vision: VEP and Psychophysical Measurements , 1991 .

[42]  R. W. Bowen Latencies for chromatic and achromatic visual mechanisms , 1981, Vision Research.

[43]  N. Graham Visual detection of aperiodic spatial stimuli by probability summation among narrowband channels , 1977, Vision Research.