Implicit scene learning is viewpoint dependent

When novel scenes are encoded, the representations of scene layout are generally viewpoint specific. Past studies of scene recognition have typically required subjects to explicitly study and encode novel scenes, but in everyday visual experience, it is possible that much scene learning occurs incidentally. Here, we examine whether implicitly encoded scene layouts are also viewpoint dependent. We used the contextual cuing paradigm, in which search for a target is facilitated by implicitly learned associations between target locations and novel spatial contexts (Chun & Jiang, 1998). This task was extended to naturalistic search arrays with apparent depth. To test viewpoint dependence, the viewpoint of the scenes was varied from training to testing. Contextual cuing and, hence, scene context learning decreased as the angular rotation from training viewpoint increased. This finding suggests that implicitly acquired representations of scene layout are viewpoint dependent.

[1]  M. Chun,et al.  Top-Down Attentional Guidance Based on Implicit Learning of Visual Covariation , 1999 .

[2]  L. Jacoby,et al.  Remembering without awareness , 1982 .

[3]  Ronald A. Rensink,et al.  TO SEE OR NOT TO SEE: The Need for Attention to Perceive Changes in Scenes , 1997 .

[4]  M. Bravo,et al.  The role of attention in different visual-search tasks , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  W. Johnston,et al.  Attention capture by novel stimuli. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[6]  tephen E. Palmer The effects of contextual scenes on the identification of objects , 1975, Memory & cognition.

[7]  D. Schacter Implicit memory: History and current status. , 1987 .

[8]  D. Shanks IMPLICIT LEARNING AND TACIT KNOWLEDGE - AN ESSAY ON THE COGNITIVE UNCONSCIOUS - REBER,A , 1995 .

[9]  H. Pashler The Psychology of Attention , 1997 .

[10]  M. Chun,et al.  Contextual Cueing: Implicit Learning and Memory of Visual Context Guides Spatial Attention , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.

[11]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  T. McNamara,et al.  Multiple views of spatial memory , 1997 .

[13]  D. Marr,et al.  Representation and recognition of the spatial organization of three-dimensional shapes , 1978, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[14]  Implicit memory for scenes guides visual exploration in the monkey , 1998 .

[15]  L. Squire Declarative and Nondeclarative Memory: Multiple Brain Systems Supporting Learning and Memory , 1992, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[16]  George Sperling,et al.  The information available in brief visual presentations. , 1960 .

[17]  Michael A. Stadler,et al.  Handbook of implicit learning , 1998 .

[18]  I. Biederman,et al.  Scene perception: Detecting and judging objects undergoing relational violations , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[19]  E. Goldstein Blackwell handbook of perception , 2001 .

[20]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[21]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: I , 1977 .

[22]  G. Logan Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .

[23]  T. McNamara,et al.  Viewpoint Dependence in Scene Recognition , 1997 .

[24]  P Cavanagh,et al.  Familiarity and pop-out in visual search , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[25]  H. Egeth,et al.  Searching for conjunctively defined targets. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  D. Simons,et al.  CHAPTER 13 – Change Blindness , 2005 .

[27]  M. Tarr Rotating objects to recognize them: A case study on the role of viewpoint dependency in the recognition of three-dimensional objects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[28]  Irwin N. Jankovic,et al.  Principles of spatial problem solving. , 1982 .

[29]  M. Chun,et al.  Implicit, long-term spatial contextual memory. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[30]  G. Logan,et al.  Memory-based automaticity in the discrimination of visual numerosity. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[31]  K. Nakayama,et al.  On the Functional Role of Implicit Visual Memory for the Adaptive Deployment of Attention Across Scenes , 2000 .

[32]  Ralph L. Rosnow,et al.  Essentials of Behavioral Research: Methods and Data Analysis , 1984 .

[33]  J Miller,et al.  Components of the location probability effect in visual search tasks. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  I. Biederman,et al.  Recognizing depth-rotated objects: Evidence and conditions for three-dimensional viewpoint invariance. , 1993 .

[35]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[36]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[37]  Deborah J. Aks,et al.  Visual search for size is influenced by a background texture gradient. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[38]  A. Kramer,et al.  Attentional guidance of the eyes by contextual information and abrupt onsets. , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[39]  T. Palmeri Exemplar similarity and the development of automaticity. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[40]  R. Shepard,et al.  Mental Images and Their Transformations , 1982 .

[41]  D. Shanks,et al.  Characteristics of dissociable human learning systems , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[42]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Implicit learning. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[43]  J. Wolfe Visual search in continuous, naturalistic stimuli , 1994, Vision Research.

[44]  H H Bülthoff,et al.  Psychophysical support for a two-dimensional view interpolation theory of object recognition. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[45]  Ranxiao Frances Wang,et al.  Perceiving Real-World Viewpoint Changes , 1998 .

[46]  M. Chun,et al.  Contextual cueing of visual attention , 2022 .

[47]  A. Reber Implicit learning and tacit knowledge , 1993 .

[48]  Marilyn L. Shaw,et al.  A capacity allocation model for reaction time. , 1978 .

[49]  A Treisman,et al.  Feature analysis in early vision: evidence from search asymmetries. , 1988, Psychological review.

[50]  Clark C. Presson,et al.  Mental rotation and the perspective problem , 1973 .

[51]  M. Chun,et al.  Selective attention modulates implicit learning , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[52]  M. Chun,et al.  Temporal contextual cuing of visual attention. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[53]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[54]  M. Chun,et al.  Perceptual constraints on implicit learning of spatial context , 2002 .

[55]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[56]  M. Shaw,et al.  Optimal allocation of cognitive resources to spatial locations. , 1977, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[57]  Allen Allport,et al.  Visual attention , 1989 .

[58]  J. Rieser Access to knowledge of spatial structure at novel points of observation. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.