Interaction of raclopride and preparatory interval effects on simple reaction time performance
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J H Wearden,et al. Scalar timing without reference memory? Episodic temporal generalization and bisection in humans , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.
[2] R. Church,et al. Methamphetamine and time estimation. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[3] B. Cooper,et al. Effects of 6-hydroxydopamine treatments on active avoidance responding: evidence for involvement of brain dopamine. , 1973, The Journal of pharmacology and experimental therapeutics.
[4] S. Sternberg. Memory-scanning: mental processes revealed by reaction-time experiments. , 1969, American scientist.
[5] T. Robbins,et al. Effects of unilateral dorsal and ventral striatal dopamine depletion on visual neglect in the rat: A neural and behavioural analysis , 1989, Neuroscience.
[6] S. B. Vincent. The function of the vibrissae in the behavior of the white rat , 1912 .
[7] W. Meck,et al. Neuropsychological mechanisms of interval timing behavior. , 2000, BioEssays : news and reviews in molecular, cellular and developmental biology.
[8] Marianne Amalric,et al. Excitotoxic lesions of the prelimbic‐infralimbic areas of the rodent prefrontal cortex disrupt motor preparatory processes , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.
[9] L. Karlin. Reaction time as a function of foreperiod duration and variability. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.
[10] Warren H. Meck,et al. Systems-level integration of interval timing and reaction time , 2004, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[11] W. Meck. Selective adjustment of the speed of internal clock and memory processes. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[12] A. Faure,et al. Lesion to the Nigrostriatal Dopamine System Disrupts Stimulus-Response Habit Formation , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[13] Ralph R. Miller,et al. Information processing in animals : memory mechanisms , 1983 .
[14] R. Church,et al. Nucleus basalis magnocellularis and medial septal area lesions differentially impair temporal memory , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[15] P. Teitelbaum,et al. Sensory Neglect Produced by Lateral Hypothalamic Damage , 1971, Science.
[16] D. Laming. Choice reaction performance following an error , 1979 .
[17] D. Blough. Reaction time signatures of discriminative processes: Differential effects of stimulus similarity and incentive , 2004, Learning & behavior.
[18] W. Hauber,et al. 6-Hydroxydopamine lesion of the rat prefrontal cortex impairs motor initiation but not motor execution , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[19] H. Helson,et al. Design of equipment and optimal human operation. , 1949, The American journal of psychology.
[20] W. Meck. Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing , 2003 .
[21] M. Nicolelis,et al. Interval timing and the encoding of signal duration by ensembles of cortical and striatal neurons. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.
[22] D Laming,et al. Autocorrelation of choice-reaction times. , 1979, Acta psychologica.
[23] W. Meck. Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[24] Dopamine D2 receptor blockade reduces response likelihood but does not affect latency to emit a learned sensory-motor response: implications for Parkinson's disease. , 2000, Behavioral neuroscience.
[25] W. Meck,et al. Differential modulation of clock speed by the administration of intermittent versus continuous cocaine. , 2004, Behavioral neuroscience.
[26] T. Rammsayer,et al. Neuropharmacological Evidence for Different Timing Mechanisms in Humans , 1999, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.
[27] Anita J. Jurkowski,et al. Variable foreperiod deficits in Parkinson’s disease: Dissociation across reflexive and voluntary behaviors , 2005, Brain and Cognition.
[28] T. Ljungberg. Attenuation of water intake and operant responding by dopamine D2 antagonists: raclopride provides important cues for understanding the functional mechanism of action. , 1989, Pharmacology & toxicology.
[29] R. Schmidt. Anticipation and timing in human motor performance. , 1968 .
[30] R. Duncan Luce,et al. Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization , 1986 .
[31] G. Koob,et al. Selective Effects of Low-Dose D2 Dopamine Receptor Antagonism in a Reaction-Time Task in Rats , 1993, Neuropsychopharmacology.
[32] Sylvan Kornblum,et al. Simple reaction time as a race between signal detection and time estimation: A paradigm and model , 1973 .
[33] Catalin V. Buhusi,et al. What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[34] R. Church,et al. Representation of time , 1990, Cognition.
[35] R. Hughes. Behavioural Mechanisms of Food Selection , 1990, NATO ASI Series.
[36] W. Meck,et al. Dissecting the Brain's Internal Clock: How Frontal–Striatal Circuitry Keeps Time and Shifts Attention , 2002, Brain and Cognition.
[37] N. Chater. The Search for Simplicity: A Fundamental Cognitive Principle? , 1999 .
[38] Joel L. Davis,et al. Adaptive Critics and the Basal Ganglia , 1995 .
[39] M. Roitman,et al. Dopamine mediation of the feeding response to violations of spatial and temporal expectancies , 2001, Behavioural Brain Research.
[40] G. Koob,et al. Depletion of dopamine in the caudate nucleus but not in nucleus accumbens impairs reaction-time performance in rats , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[41] D A Rosenbaum,et al. Timing and reaction time. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[42] R M Church,et al. Scalar Timing in Memory , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[43] T. Robbins,et al. Depletion of unilateral striatal dopamine impairs initiation of contralateral actions and not sensory attention , 1985, Nature.
[44] R. Church,et al. Scalar expectancy theory and choice between delayed rewards. , 1988, Psychological review.
[45] C. Buhusi,et al. Differential effects of methamphetamine and haloperidol on the control of an internal clock. , 2002, Behavioral neuroscience.
[46] P. J. Brasted,et al. The effect of systemic d-amphetamine on motor versus motivational processes in the rat , 1996, Psychopharmacology.
[47] Brain and mind : evolutionary perspectives , 1998 .
[48] P. Teitelbaum,et al. Further analysis of sensory inattention following lateral hypothalamic damage in rats. , 1974, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[49] T. Rammsayer,et al. Are there dissociable roles of the mesostriatal and mesolimbocortical dopamine systems on temporal information processing in humans? , 1997, Neuropsychobiology.
[50] W. Meck,et al. Differential effects of clozapine and haloperidol on interval timing in the supraseconds range , 2005, Psychopharmacology.
[51] H. M. Johnson. Reaction-time measurements. , 2022 .
[52] R S Nickerson,et al. Response Time to the Second of Two Successive Signals as a Function of Absolute and Relative Duration of Intersignal Interval , 1965, Perceptual and motor skills.
[53] P. Killeen,et al. A behavioral theory of timing. , 1988, Psychological review.
[54] T. Robbins,et al. Simple and choice reaction time performance following unilateral striatal dopamine depletion in the rat. Impaired motor readiness but preserved response preparation. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[55] S. Nakajima,et al. Effects of D2 dopamine receptor blockade with raclopride on intracranial self-stimulation and food-reinforced operant behaviour , 2004, Psychopharmacology.
[56] T. Robbins,et al. Striatal lesions produce distinctive impairments in reaction time performance in two different operant chambers , 1998, Brain Research Bulletin.
[57] D. Anger. The dependence of interresponse times upon the relative reinforcement of different interresponse times. , 1956, Journal of experimental psychology.
[58] R. Ratcliff,et al. A comparison of macaque behavior and superior colliculus neuronal activity to predictions from models of two-choice decisions. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[59] J. Horvitz,et al. Effects of dopamine antagonists on the timing of two intervals , 2003, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.
[60] C. B. Ferster,et al. Schedules of reinforcement , 1957 .
[61] R. Church,et al. Application of scalar timing theory to individual trials. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[62] R Näätänen,et al. The diminishing time-uncertainty with the lapse of time after the warning signal in reaction-time experiments with varying fore-periods. , 1970, Acta psychologica.
[63] E. C. Poulton,et al. Perceptual Anticipation and Reaction Time , 1950 .
[64] R. Ratcliff. Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics. , 1979, Psychological bulletin.
[65] W. H. Teichner. Recent studies of simple reaction time. , 1954, Psychological bulletin.
[66] Warren H. Meck,et al. Chronic treatment with haloperidol induces deficits in working memory and feedback effects of interval timing , 2005, Brain and Cognition.
[67] J. Gibbon,et al. Scalar expectancy theory and peak-interval timing in humans. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[68] M. Çevik. Effects of methamphetamine on duration discrimination. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.
[69] W. Meck. Neuropsychology of timing and time perception , 2005, Brain and Cognition.
[70] J. Horvitz,et al. Extended Habit Training Reduces Dopamine Mediation of Appetitive Response Expression , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[71] E. T. Klemmer. Time uncertainty in simple reaction time. , 1956, Journal of experimental psychology.
[72] C. Gallistel,et al. Computational Versus Associative Models of Simple Conditioning , 2001 .
[73] Rolf Ulrich,et al. Locus of the effect of temporal preparation: evidence from the lateralized readiness potential. , 2003, Psychophysiology.
[74] A. Nieoullon,et al. Dopamine and complex sensorimotor integration: Further studies in a conditioned motor task in the rat , 1995, Neuroscience.
[75] J. Gibbon. Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. , 1977 .
[76] Alex Kacelnik,et al. Optimal foraging and timing processes in the starling, Sturnus vulgaris: effect of inter-capture interval , 1992, Animal Behaviour.
[77] W. Meck. Neuroanatomical localization of an internal clock: A functional link between mesolimbic, nigrostriatal, and mesocortical dopaminergic systems , 2006, Brain Research.
[78] J Gibbon,et al. Mnemonics for variability: remembering food delay. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[79] R. Woodworth,et al. PSYCHIATRY AND EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY , 1906 .
[80] J. Gibbon,et al. Memory for inter-reinforcement interval variability and patch departure decisions in the starling, Sturnus vulgaris , 1996, Animal Behaviour.
[81] C. Gallistel,et al. Toward a neurobiology of temporal cognition: advances and challenges , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[82] W. Meck,et al. Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: coincidence detection of oscillatory processes. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[83] M. J. Billington,et al. The Deadline model for simple reaction times , 1972 .
[84] A. Phillips,et al. Deficits in instrumental responding after 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the nigro-neostriatal dopaminergic projection. , 1974, Pharmacology, biochemistry, and behavior.
[85] R. Näätänen,et al. Foreperiod and simple reaction time. , 1981 .
[86] Warren H. Meck,et al. Frontal cortex lesions eliminate the clock speed effect of dopaminergic drugs on interval timing , 2006, Brain Research.
[87] M. Bateson,et al. Single-trials analyses demonstrate that increases in clock speed contribute to the methamphetamine-induced horizontal shifts in peak-interval timing functions , 2006, Psychopharmacology.
[88] J. Gibbon,et al. Coupled Temporal Memories in Parkinson's Disease: A Dopamine-Related Dysfunction , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[89] Arjan Blokland,et al. Reaction Time Responding in Rats , 1998, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[90] M. Manosevitz,et al. High-Speed Scanning in Human Memory , 2022 .
[91] L. Bherer,et al. Please Scroll down for Article Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology Temporal Preparation Strategy May Inflate Rt Deficit in Patients with Parkinson's Disease , 2022 .
[92] Y. Sakurai,et al. Effect of 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the medial prefrontal cortex of the rat on performance in a reaction time task , 1995, European Neuropsychopharmacology.
[93] A. Barzilai,et al. Molecular mechanisms of selective dopaminergic neuronal death in Parkinson's disease. , 2003, Trends in molecular medicine.
[94] Roger Ratcliff,et al. A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .
[95] Christopher D. Adams. Variations in the Sensitivity of Instrumental Responding to Reinforcer Devaluation , 1982 .
[96] H. Hall,et al. The selective dopamine D2 receptor antagonist raclopride discriminates between dopamine-mediated motor functions , 2004, Psychopharmacology.
[97] P. Overton,et al. Disruption of conditioned reaction time performance by dopamine receptor antagonists in the rat , 1993, Behavioural pharmacology.
[98] John Gibbon,et al. Timing Mechanisms in Optimal Foraging: Some Applications of Scalar Expectancy Theory , 1990 .
[99] Stephen J. Boies,et al. Components of attention. , 1971 .
[100] Warren H. Meck,et al. Affinity for the dopamine D2 receptor predicts neuroleptic potency in decreasing the speed of an internal clock , 1986, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.
[101] James C. Houk,et al. Information Processing in Modular Circuits Linking Basal Ganglia and Cerebral Cortex , 1994 .