Goal-Dependent and Goal-Independent Effects of Irrelevant Evaluations

Participants responded to pairs of words under different processing goals. In the first four experiments, they identified different attributes of the second word of each pair. Priming effects of irrelevant evaluations occurred if and only if the goal was to identify the valence of the second word. Equivalent goal-dependent priming effects were found for irrelevant nonevaluative attributes of the word pairs. In the second set of four experiments, participants made same-different comparisons on the word pairs. Irrespective of which (nonevaluative) attributes were task-relevant, effects of irrelevant evaluations occurred. Equivalent goal-independent effects of irrelevant, nonevaluative attributes were not found. A ninth study found crucial effects of both series simultaneously. The results bear on the issues of goal dependence of affective priming and automatic attitude activation, of whether affective processing differs from cognitive processing, and of the mechanisms underlying the effects of irrelevant evaluations.

[1]  M. Banaji,et al.  When fair is foul and foul is fair: reverse priming in automatic evaluation. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[2]  S C Draine,et al.  Replicable unconscious semantic priming. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[3]  A. D. Groot,et al.  Primed Lexical Decision: Combined Effects of the Proportion of Related Prime-Target Pairs and the Stimulus-Onset Asynchrony of Prime and Target , 1984 .

[4]  S. Chaiken,et al.  The Automatic Evaluation Effect: Unconditional Automatic Attitude Activation with a Pronunciation Task , 1996 .

[5]  J. H. Neely Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. , 1991 .

[6]  R. Fazio,et al.  On the costs of accessible attitudes: detecting that the attitude object has changed. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[7]  Richard L. Abrams,et al.  Three Cognitive Markers of Unconscious Semantic Activation , 1996, Science.

[8]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[9]  Mich Schwibbe,et al.  Zum emotionalen Gehalt von Substantiven, Adjektiven und Verben. , 1981 .

[10]  M. Klinger,et al.  Mechanisms of unconscious priming: I. Response competition, not spreading activation. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[11]  E. Fox Negative priming from ignored distractors in visual selection: A review , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[12]  E. Stern,et al.  How attitudes guide memory-based judgments: A two-process model , 1992 .

[13]  J. Bargh The automaticity of everyday life. , 1997 .

[14]  K. C. Klauer,et al.  Locational uncertainty moderates affective congruency effects in the evaluative decision task , 2001 .

[15]  C. Judd,et al.  Evaluative versus Conceptual Judgments in Automatic Stereotyping and Prejudice , 2001 .

[16]  M. Banaji,et al.  Automatic and Controlled Processes in Stereotype Priming , 1996 .

[17]  I. Blair,et al.  The Malleability of Automatic Stereotypes and Prejudice , 2002 .

[18]  K. C. Klauer,et al.  List-context effects in evaluative priming. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[19]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Categorical and contextual bases of person memory and stereotyping. , 1978 .

[20]  Jan De Houwer,et al.  An Affective Variant of the Simon Paradigm , 1998 .

[21]  Odmar Neumann,et al.  Automatic Processing: A Review of Recent Findings and a Plea for an Old Theory , 1984 .

[22]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Personality and Social Psychology Bulle- Tin Chen, Bargh / Consequences of Automatic Evaluation Immediate Behavioral Predispositions to Approach or Avoid the Stimulus , 2022 .

[23]  D. Hermans,et al.  Affective priming using a color-naming task: a test of an affective-motivational account of affective priming effects. , 1998, Zeitschrift fur experimentelle Psychologie : Organ der Deutschen Gesellschaft fur Psychologie.

[24]  K C Klauer,et al.  Does Sunshine Prime Loyal? Affective Priming in the Naming Task , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[25]  R. Zajonc,et al.  Affect, cognition, and awareness: affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[26]  K. C. Klauer,et al.  The Psychology of Evaluation : Affective Processes in Cognition and Emotion , 2003 .

[27]  D. Holender,et al.  Expectancy effects, congruity effects and the interpretation of response latency measurement , 1992 .

[28]  Eliot R. Smith Mental representation and memory. , 1998 .

[29]  S. Tipper,et al.  Behavioural Goals Determine Inhibitory Mechanisms of Selective Attention , 1994 .

[30]  D. Hermans,et al.  The affective priming effect: Automatic activation of evaluative information in memory. , 1994 .

[31]  R. Zajonc Feeling and thinking : Preferences need no inferences , 1980 .

[32]  G. Logan,et al.  When it helps to be misled: Facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop-like task , 1979 .

[33]  B. Farell,et al.  Same-different judgments: a review of current controversies in perceptual comparisons. , 1985, Psychological bulletin.

[34]  D. Wentura,et al.  [A fair test of the spreading activation hypothesis: a study of affective congruency effects in the Stroop test]. , 1998, Zeitschrift fur experimentelle Psychologie : Organ der Deutschen Gesellschaft fur Psychologie.