The effects of unitization on familiarity-based source memory: testing a behavioral prediction derived from neuroimaging data.

Performance on tests of source memory is typically based on recollection of contextual information associated with an item. However, recent neuroimaging results have suggested that the perirhinal cortex, a region thought to support familiarity-based item recognition, may support source attributions if source information is encoded as a feature of the relevant item (i.e., "unitized"). The authors hypothesized that familiarity may contribute to source memory performance if item and source information are unitized during encoding, whereas performance may rely more heavily on recollection if source information is encoded as an arbitrary contextual association. Three source recognition experiments examining receiver operating characteristics and response deadline performance indicated that familiarity makes a greater contribution to source memory if source and item information are unitized during encoding. These findings suggest that familiarity can contribute to source recognition and that its contribution depends critically on the way item and source information are initially processed.

[1]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  A model for recognition memory: REM—retrieving effectively from memory , 1997, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[2]  Malcolm W. Brown,et al.  Contrasting Hippocampal and Perirhinalcortex Function using Immediate Early Gene Imaging , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[3]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Multiple routes to memory: Distinct medial temporal lobe processes build item and source memories , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[4]  Joel L. Voss,et al.  Validating neural correlates of familiarity , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Familiarity breeds differentiation: a subjective-likelihood approach to the effects of experience in recognition memory. , 1998, Psychological review.

[6]  Lynne M. Reder,et al.  Models of recognition: A review of arguments in favor of a dual-process account , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[7]  Sabrina M. Tom,et al.  Dissociable correlates of recollection and familiarity within the medial temporal lobes , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[9]  Ellen M. Migo,et al.  Associative memory and the medial temporal lobes , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  A P Yonelinas,et al.  The contribution of recollection and familiarity to recognition and source-memory judgments: a formal dual-process model and an analysis of receiver operating characteristics. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[11]  Daniel L Schacter,et al.  Amygdala Activity Is Associated with the Successful Encoding of Item, But Not Source, Information for Positive and Negative Stimuli , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  A. Yonelinas The Nature of Recollection and Familiarity: A Review of 30 Years of Research , 2002 .

[13]  David A. Caulton,et al.  Recognition Memory and Modality Judgments: A Comparison of Retrieval Dynamics , 1997 .

[14]  Lila Davachi,et al.  Behavioral / Systems / Cognitive Differential Encoding Mechanisms for Subsequent Associative Recognition and Free Recall , 2012 .

[15]  Tim Curran,et al.  Retrieval constraints and the mirror effect , 1994 .

[16]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .

[17]  L L Jacoby,et al.  Isolating the contributions of familiarity and source information to item recognition: a time course analysis. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[18]  N. Stanietsky,et al.  The interaction of TIGIT with PVR and PVRL2 inhibits human NK cell cytotoxicity , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[19]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  The hippocampus--what does it do? , 1992, Behavioral and neural biology.

[20]  Colleen M. Parks,et al.  Moving beyond pure signal-detection models: comment on Wixted (2007). , 2007, Psychological review.

[21]  Murray Glanzer,et al.  Six regularities of source recognition. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[22]  J. Wixted Dual-process theory and signal-detection theory of recognition memory. , 2007, Psychological review.

[23]  M. Verfaellie,et al.  The contribution of familiarity to associative memory in amnesia , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  J. Bartlett,et al.  Inversion and processing of component and spatial-relational information in faces. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  A P Yonelinas,et al.  Recognition memory for faces: When familiarity supports associative recognition judgments , 1999, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[26]  C. L. Raye,et al.  Source ROCs are (typically) curvilinear: comment on Yonelinas (1999). , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[27]  H Eichenbaum,et al.  Memory for items and memory for relations in the procedural/declarative memory framework. , 1997, Memory.

[28]  A. Yonelinas Receiver-operating characteristics in recognition memory: evidence for a dual-process model. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[29]  A. Mecklinger,et al.  Intra- and Inter-Item Associations Doubly Dissociate the Electrophysiological Correlates of Familiarity and Recollection , 2006, Neuron.

[30]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Unitization and grouping mediate dissociations in memory for new , 1989 .

[31]  K. Norman,et al.  Effect of unitization on associative recognition in amnesia , 2007, Hippocampus.

[32]  Melina R. Uncapher,et al.  Episodic Encoding Is More than the Sum of Its Parts: An fMRI Investigation of Multifeatural Contextual Encoding , 2006, Neuron.

[33]  Guillén Fernández,et al.  Process dissociation between contextual retrieval and item recognition , 2004, Neuroreport.

[34]  W. Scoville,et al.  LOSS OF RECENT MEMORY AFTER BILATERAL HIPPOCAMPAL LESIONS , 1957, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[35]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Two functional components of the hippocampal memory system , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[36]  Scott D. Slotnick,et al.  Support for a continuous (single-process) model of recognition memory and source memory , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[37]  John P. Aggleton,et al.  Interleaving brain systems for episodic and recognition memory , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  David I. Donaldson,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence for the influence of unitization on the processes engaged during episodic retrieval: Enhancing familiarity based remembering , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  Bertram Opitz,et al.  Contribution of Familiarity and Recollection to Associative Recognition Memory: Insights from Event-related Potentials , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  Rachel A. Diana,et al.  Imaging recollection and familiarity in the medial temporal lobe: a three-component model , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.