Indispensable benefits and unavoidable costs of unattended sound for cognitive functioning.

Critical to survival, and also to the organism's efficient management of the flow of information in the brain, is attentional selectivity; the ability to select one source of information to guide action whilst ignoring others that are irrelevant to the current behavioural goal. But such selectivity is not merely the inclusion of the relevant information and the complete neglect of irrelevant information. We discuss in this paper the way that all sound is processed in an obligatory fashion--whether relevant or irrelevant--and discuss the fate of sound in the case when it is irrelevant to the immediate mental task. Using the so-called irrelevant sound paradigm we show that unattended information is both registered and organised. This obligatory process of organisation compromises the efficiency of particular types of mental activity. We discuss how such interference comes about but the key emphasis is upon the possible beneficial effects of such processing-of-the-irrelevant, in allowing the switching of attention to be more facile and intelligent and in allowing the accumulation of evidence about statistical regularities in the auditory world (such as those helpful to the efficient perception, acquisition and use of language). In sum, we describe how purposeful processing based on directed attention is in a state of tension with the obligatory, automatic processing of the unattended. One of the consequences of this tension is typically manifested in auditory distraction, but the benefits of processing of the attended may considerably outweigh this disadvantage.

[1]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  On the Irrelevance of Semantic Information for the “Irrelevant Speech” Effect , 1996 .

[2]  A. Allport Attention and control: have we been asking the wrong questions? A critical review of twenty-five years , 1993 .

[3]  Deborah M. Shaibe,et al.  On the Irrelevance of Phonological Similarity to the Irrelevant Speech Effect , 1997, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[4]  K. Lashley The problem of serial order in behavior , 1951 .

[5]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Privileged Access by Irrelevant Speech to Short-term Memory: The Role of Changing State , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[6]  T. A. Cartwright,et al.  Distributional regularity and phonotactic constraints are useful for segmentation , 1996, Cognition.

[7]  Allen Allport,et al.  Visual attention , 1989 .

[8]  J. Duncan Selective attention and the organization of visual information. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[9]  Albert S. Bregman,et al.  The Auditory Scene. (Book Reviews: Auditory Scene Analysis. The Perceptual Organization of Sound.) , 1990 .

[10]  D. Berry,et al.  Disruption of office‐related tasks by speech and office noise , 1998 .

[11]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Irrelevant tones produce an irrelevant speech effect : Implications for phonological coding in working memory , 1993 .

[12]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  The intrusiveness of sound: Laboratory findings and their implications for noise abatement. , 2001, Noise & health.

[13]  W. Prinz,et al.  Chapter 5 Involuntary attention , 1996 .

[14]  H. Pashler The Psychology of Attention , 1997 .

[15]  R. Henson Short-Term Memory for Serial Order: The Start-End Model , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.

[16]  T. Mondor,et al.  Mechanisms of perceptual organization and auditory selective attention: the role of pattern structure. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  W. Ellermeier,et al.  Individual differences in susceptibility to the "irrelevant speech effect". , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[18]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  A negative order-repetition priming effect: inhibition of order in unattended auditory sequences? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  Jon Driver,et al.  On the nonselectivity of "selective" seeing: Contrasts between interference and priming in selective attention. , 1989 .

[20]  Nick Mosdell,et al.  The role of habituation in the disruption of recall performance by irrelevant sound , 1997 .

[21]  E. N. Solokov Perception and the conditioned reflex , 1963 .

[22]  A. Parkin,et al.  Human memory , 1999, Current Biology.

[23]  Alan B. Welsh,et al.  Acoustic masking in primary memory. , 1976 .

[24]  Jessica M. Foxton,et al.  Effects of attention and unilateral neglect on auditory stream segregation. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  Alexander I. Rudnicky,et al.  Auditory segregation: stream or streams? , 1975, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  Kevin J. Hawley,et al.  Perceptual inhibition of expected inputs: The key that opens closed minds , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[27]  S Tremblay,et al.  Change of intensity fails to produce an irrelevant sound effect: implications for the representation of unattended sound. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Irrelevant sound disrupts order information in free recall as in serial recall. , 1998 .

[29]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Objects, streams, and threads of auditory attention. , 1993 .

[30]  Dylan Marc Jones The cognitive psychology of auditory distraction: The 1997 BPS Broadbent Lecture , 1999 .

[31]  Peter W. Jusczyk,et al.  How infants begin to extract words from speech , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  I. Thomas,et al.  Temporal order in the perception of vowels. , 1970, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[33]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Phonological similarity in the irrelevant speech effect: Within- or between-stream similarity? , 1995 .

[34]  Clive Frankish,et al.  Perceptual organization and precategorical acoustic storage. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[35]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Organizational factors in the effect of irrelevant speech: The role of spatial location and timing , 1995, Memory & cognition.

[36]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Role of Serial Order in the Irrelevant Speech Effect: Tests of the Changing-State Hypothesis , 1997 .

[37]  N. Cowan Attention and Memory: An Integrated Framework , 1995 .

[38]  C. P. Beaman,et al.  The object-oriented episodic record model , 1996 .

[39]  Dylan Marc Jones,et al.  1 – Noise and Performance , 1992 .

[40]  E. Newport,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article INCIDENTAL LANGUAGE LEARNING: Ustening (and Learning) out of the Comer of Your Ear , 2022 .

[41]  Alan D. Baddeley,et al.  Effects of Background Music on Phonological Short-Term Memory , 1989 .

[42]  D. Broadbent Perception and communication , 1958 .

[43]  E. Newport,et al.  WORD SEGMENTATION : THE ROLE OF DISTRIBUTIONAL CUES , 1996 .

[44]  Herbert A. Colle,et al.  Auditory encoding in visual short-term recall: effects of noise intensity and spatial location , 1980 .

[45]  David L. Strayer,et al.  15 - A Dynamic, Evolutionary Perspective on Attention Capture1 , 2001 .

[46]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Irrelevant Sound Disrupts Order Information in Free Recall as in Serial Recall , 1998, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[47]  S. Tipper,et al.  A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. , 1994 .

[48]  R. Näätänen Attention and brain function , 1992 .

[49]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Modulation of the Irrelevant Sound Effect by Organizational Factors: Further Evidence from Streaming by Location , 1999 .

[50]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Does auditory streaming require attention? Evidence from attentional selectivity in short-term memory. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[51]  R. G. Crowder Principles of learning and memory , 1977 .

[52]  Z. Harris,et al.  Methods in structural linguistics. , 1952 .

[53]  Alan D. Baddeley,et al.  Disruption of short-term memory by unattended speech : Implications for the structure of working memory , 1982 .

[54]  Seiichiro Namba,et al.  Irrelevant background speech and human performance: Is there long-term habituation? , 1996 .

[55]  Albert S. Bregman,et al.  Auditory scene analysis : hearing in complex environments , 1993 .

[56]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Interference in memory by process or content? A reply to Neath (2000) , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[57]  S. Tipper The Negative Priming Effect: Inhibitory Priming by Ignored Objects , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[58]  Shaun P. Vecera,et al.  Toward a Biased Competition Account of Object-Based Segregation and Attention , 2000 .

[59]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Organizational factors in selective attention: The interplay of acoustic distinctiveness and auditory streaming in the irrelevant sound effect , 1999 .

[60]  Michael W. Eysenck,et al.  Theories of Attention , 1982 .

[61]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[62]  Sébastien Tremblay,et al.  Attentional Selectivity in Short-term Memory: Similarity of Process, Not Similarity of Content, Determines Disruption , 1999 .

[63]  D C LeCompte,et al.  Extending the irrelevant speech effect beyond serial recall. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[64]  S. Tipper,et al.  Selective Attention and Priming: Inhibitory and Facilitatory Effects of Ignored Primes , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[65]  W. Levelt,et al.  Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .

[66]  Bill Macken,et al.  Role of Habituation in the Irrelevant Sound Effect : Evidence From the Effects of Token Set Size and Rate of Transition , 2001 .

[67]  Dianne C. Berry,et al.  Habituation and dishabituation to speech and office noise , 1997 .

[68]  S. Tipper Does Negative Priming Reflect Inhibitory Mechanisms? A Review and Integration of Conflicting Views , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.