Unconscious pop-out: Attentional capture by unseen feature singletons only when top-down attention is available

Visual “pop-out” occurs when a unique visual target (e.g. a feature singleton) is present among a set of homogeneous distractors. However, the role of visual awareness in this process remains unclear. Here we show that, even though subjects were not aware of a suppressed pop-out display, their subsequent performance on an orientation discrimination task was significantly better at the pop-out location than at a control location. These results indicate that visual awareness of a feature singleton is not necessary for it to attract attention. Furthermore, our results show that the subliminal pop-out effect disappeared when subjects diverted their attention toward an RSVP task while viewing the same subliminal pop-out display, suggesting that the availability of top-down attention is necessary for the subliminal pop-out effect, and that the cognitive processes underlying attention and awareness are somewhat independent. Visual “pop-out” refers to the phenomenon in which a unique visual target (e.g. a feature singleton) can be rapidly detected among a set of homogeneous distractors (Treisman, 1985; Wolfe, 1994). Here we inquire about the roles of visual awareness and attention in this process. Specifically, what actually makes a feature singleton pop out? Is awareness of the feature singleton necessary for the stimulus to attract attention? Or can a feature singleton be processed and summon attention without the subject ever being aware of the stimulus?1 We investigated (1) whether visual awareness of a feature singleton is necessary for that stimulus to capture attention and (2) whether top-down attention is necessary to elicit such a subliminal response. Using the continuous flash suppression paradigm (Tsuchiya and Koch, 2005), we presented a pop-out display (a feature singleton among several distractors) subliminally and demonstrated that, even though subjects were not aware of the display, their subsequent performance on an orientation discrimination task was significantly better at the pop-out location than at a control location (Experiment 1). Furthermore, we showed that this effect disappeared when subjects diverted their attention toward a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) task while viewing the same subliminal pop-out display (Experiment 2). Together, these findings suggest that (1) awareness of a feature singleton is not necessary for it to summon attention and that (2) top-down attention is necessary for this subliminal effect to occur. *To whom correspondence should be addressed. hsieh.pj@gmail.com. 1Awareness is operationalized here as reportability (Dehaene et al., 2006). Top-down attention has been related to spatial, featural, temporal, and object-based variants of attention behaviorally, and neuronal ‘versions’ of top-down attention also include ‘shrinking receptive fields’ (Bundesen et al., 2005). For our purposes, top-down attention here only refers to the spatial and featural variants of attention. NIH Public Access

[1]  L. Zhaoping Attention capture by eye of origin singletons even without awareness--a hallmark of a bottom-up saliency map in the primary visual cortex. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[2]  Ken Nakayama,et al.  Attentional requirements in a ‘preattentive’ feature search task , 1997, Nature.

[3]  Lamme Vaf,et al.  Why visual attention and awareness are different , 2003 .

[4]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: a critical review of visual masking , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[5]  R. W. Kentridge,et al.  Attended but unseen: Visual attention is not sufficient for visual awareness , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  D. Sagi,et al.  Visual attention and perceptual grouping , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[7]  C. Koch,et al.  Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[8]  Philip M. Merikle,et al.  Parallels between Perception without Attention and Perception without Awareness , 1997, Consciousness and Cognition.

[9]  P. McCormick Orienting attention without awareness. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  C. Koch,et al.  Computational modelling of visual attention , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[11]  G. Boynton,et al.  Capture of Attention to Threatening Stimuli without Perceptual Awareness , 2009, Current Biology.

[12]  Charles Spence,et al.  Perceptual load affects exogenous spatial orienting while working memory load does not , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  Linda Ng Boyle,et al.  The Interaction of Cognitive Load and Attention-Directing Cues in Driving , 2009, Hum. Factors.

[14]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  The size of an attentional window modulates attentional capture by color singletons , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[15]  Geraint Rees,et al.  Perceptual load modulates conscious flicker perception. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[16]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[17]  Andrew B. Leber,et al.  It’s under control: Top-down search strategies can override attentional capture , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[18]  Fang Fang,et al.  A gender- and sexual orientation-dependent spatial attentional effect of invisible images , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[19]  C. Bundesen,et al.  A neural theory of visual attention: bridging cognition and neurophysiology. , 2005, Psychological review.

[20]  C. Tallon-Baudry,et al.  Neural Dissociation between Visual Awareness and Spatial Attention , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  B. Bahrami,et al.  Attentional Load Modulates Responses of Human Primary Visual Cortex to Invisible Stimuli , 2007, Current Biology.

[22]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  The Scope and Limits of Top-Down Attention in Unconscious Visual Processing , 2006, Current Biology.

[23]  M. Wolbarsht Colour Vision: Physiology and Psychophysics. The Cambridge Colour Vision Conference, 1982. J. D. Mollon , L. T. Sharpe , 1985 .

[24]  H. Egeth,et al.  Perception without attention: evidence of grouping under conditions of inattention. , 1997, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.

[25]  S Ullman,et al.  Shifts in selective visual attention: towards the underlying neural circuitry. , 1985, Human neurobiology.

[26]  Gaia Scerif,et al.  Subliminally Presented and Stored Objects Capture Spatial Attention , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  J. Changeux,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.10 No.5 May 2006 Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a testable taxonomy , 2022 .

[28]  Geraint Rees,et al.  Unconscious orientation processing depends on perceptual load. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[29]  I. Rock,et al.  Inattentional blindness: Perception without attention. , 1998 .

[30]  A. Treisman Preattentive processing in vision , 1985, Comput. Vis. Graph. Image Process..

[31]  D. Sagi,et al.  Texture-Based Tasks are Little Affected by Second Tasks Requiring Peripheral or Central Attentive Fixation , 1991, Perception.

[32]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Unconscious Masked Priming Depends on Temporal Attention , 2002, Psychological science.

[33]  Carly J. Leonard,et al.  The role of attention in subitizing: Is the magical number 1? , 2008 .

[34]  Zhaoping Li A saliency map in primary visual cortex , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[35]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Imaging unconscious semantic priming , 1998, Nature.

[36]  D. Sagi,et al.  Vision outside the focus of attention , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[37]  R. W. Kentridge,et al.  Spatial attention speeds discrimination without awareness in blindsight , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  A. Noë,et al.  A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[39]  C. Koch,et al.  Attention and consciousness: two distinct brain processes , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.