Investigating the nature of children's altruism using a social humanoid robot

Abstract Young children help people achieve their goals in various situations, even in the absence of verbal requests or rewards. Such help is typically described as “indiscriminate” because children's help is robust across many aspects of the recipient in need. However, as previous studies have almost entirely focused on human recipients, the question arises as to how indiscriminate children's helping behaviour really is. Here, we aimed to investigate whether young children's helping behaviour would extend to a nonhuman entity—a humanoid robot. In an experimental between-subjects design, 40 children witnessed a robot drop an object out of its reach either seemingly intentionally or unintentionally. Results showed that children typically returned the object, but only when it was dropped apparently unintentionally—indicating that children attribute goals to a robot, understand when it requires help, and are motivated to help it. Results of a post-test animacy interview were consistent with this interpretation as children attributed affective, cognitive, and physiological characteristics to the robot. We conclude that the indiscriminate nature of young children's altruistic behaviour extends to humanoid robots. This finding has important theoretical and practical implications for the future development of social robots as well as for the study of children's prosocial behaviour.

[1]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[2]  Leon Sterling,et al.  Robots in Rehab: Towards socially assistive robots for paediatric rehabilitation , 2015, OZCHI.

[3]  S. Baron-Cohen Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind , 1997 .

[4]  Tony Belpaeme,et al.  Comparing Robot Embodiments in a Guided Discovery Learning Interaction with Children , 2015, Int. J. Soc. Robotics.

[5]  T. Kanda,et al.  How to Build an Intentional Android: Infants' Imitation of a Robot's Goal-Directed Actions , 2008 .

[6]  C. Daniel Batson,et al.  Is Empathic Emotion a Source of Altruistic Motivation , 1981 .

[7]  Brian Scassellati,et al.  Effects of form and motion on judgments of social robots' animacy, likability, trustworthiness and unpleasantness , 2016, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[8]  T. Kanda,et al.  Six-and-a-half-month-old children positively attribute goals to human action and to humanoid-robot motion , 2005 .

[9]  Stephanie M. Curenton,et al.  Children's Understanding of the Animacy Constraint on Pretense. , 2000 .

[10]  A. Woodward Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach , 1998, Cognition.

[11]  Yuyan Luo,et al.  Three-month-old infants attribute goals to a non-human agent. , 2011, Developmental science.

[12]  J. Hamlin,et al.  Social evaluation by preverbal infants , 2007, Nature.

[13]  Peter Robinson,et al.  How anthropomorphism affects empathy toward robots , 2009, 2009 4th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI).

[14]  Michiteru Kitazaki,et al.  Measuring empathy for human and robot hand pain using electroencephalography , 2015, Scientific Reports.

[15]  H. L. Rheingold Little Children's Participation in the Work of Adults, a Nascent Prosocial Behavior. , 1982 .

[16]  Tony Belpaeme,et al.  Child-Robot Interaction: Perspectives and Challenges , 2013, ICSR.

[17]  G. Csibra Goal attribution to inanimate agents by 6.5-month-old infants , 2008, Cognition.

[18]  C. Brownell,et al.  Toddlers' prosocial behavior: from instrumental to empathic to altruistic helping. , 2010, Child development.

[19]  Tanya N. Beran,et al.  Would Children Help a Robot in Need? , 2011, Int. J. Soc. Robotics.

[20]  R. Baillargeon,et al.  Can a Self-Propelled Box Have a Goal? , 2005, Psychological science.

[21]  Ben Kenward,et al.  Infants Help a Non-Human Agent , 2013, PloS one.

[22]  Martin Buss,et al.  Increasing Helpfulness towards a Robot by Emotional Adaption to the User , 2013, International Journal of Social Robotics.

[23]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Toddlers Help Anonymously , 2017 .

[24]  Tanya N. Beran,et al.  Understanding how children understand robots: Perceived animism in child-robot interaction , 2011, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[25]  Leila Takayama,et al.  Help Me Please: Robot Politeness Strategies for Soliciting Help From Humans , 2016, CHI.

[26]  Yuyan Luo,et al.  The attribution of attention: 9-month-olds' interpretation of gaze as goal-directed action. , 2007, Developmental science.

[27]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  The emergence of contingent reciprocity in young children. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[28]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Helping and Cooperation at 14 Months of Age. , 2007, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[29]  Toddlers prefer to help familiar people. , 2018, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[30]  Brian Scassellati,et al.  Improving social skills in children with ASD using a long-term, in-home social robot , 2018, Science Robotics.

[31]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Children's Intrinsic Motivation to Provide Help Themselves After Accidentally Harming Others. , 2016, Child development.

[32]  Gerald Bieber,et al.  Perspectives on Social Robots: From the Historic Background to an Experts' View on Future Developments , 2018, PETRA.

[33]  Riccardo Campa The rise of social robots : a review of the recent literature , 2016 .

[34]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Young Children Are Intrinsically Motivated to See Others Helped , 2012, Psychological science.

[35]  M. Tomasello,et al.  The roots of human altruism. , 2009, British journal of psychology.

[36]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Unwilling versus unable: infants' understanding of intentional action. , 2005, Developmental psychology.

[37]  Christoph Bartneck,et al.  “Daisy, daisy, give me your answer do!” switching off a robot , 2007, 2007 2nd ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI).

[38]  C. Batson,et al.  Empathic mediation of helping: A two-stage model. , 1978 .

[39]  Fabio Tesser,et al.  Multimodal child-robot interaction: building social bonds , 2013, HRI 2013.

[40]  J. Campos,et al.  Do young toddlers act on their social preferences? , 2013, Developmental psychology.

[41]  D. Picard,et al.  Further Evidence for Infants' Preference for Prosocial Over Antisocial Behaviors , 2015 .

[42]  Marina Fridin,et al.  Storytelling by a kindergarten social assistive robot: A tool for constructive learning in preschool education , 2014, Comput. Educ..

[43]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Spontaneous Altruism by Chimpanzees and Young Children , 2007, PLoS biology.

[44]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Parental presence and encouragement do not influence helping in young children , 2013 .

[45]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Toddlers Help a Peer. , 2017, Child development.

[46]  Tamas Haidegger,et al.  Human-Robot Interaction : Safety, Standardization, and Benchmarking , 2019 .

[47]  Hiroshi Ishiguro Studies on Humanlike Robots - Humanoid, Android and Geminoid , 2008, SIMPAR.

[48]  Brian T. Gill,et al.  "Robovie, you'll have to go into the closet now": children's social and moral relationships with a humanoid robot. , 2012, Developmental psychology.

[49]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Extrinsic rewards undermine altruistic tendencies in 20-month-olds. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[50]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Sympathy through Affective Perspective Taking and Its Relation to Prosocial Behavior in Toddlers Sachmerda for Help with Coding; Roger Mundry for Statistical Advice; And , 2022 .

[51]  V. Kuhlmeier,et al.  Classifying prosocial behavior: children's responses to instrumental need, emotional distress, and material desire. , 2013, Child development.

[52]  D. Premack The infant's theory of self-propelled objects , 1990, Cognition.

[53]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Altruistic Helping in Human Infants and Young Chimpanzees , 2006, Science.

[54]  Sonya S. Kwak,et al.  Can robotic emotional expressions induce a human to empathize with a robot? , 2009, RO-MAN 2009 - The 18th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication.

[55]  H. Wellman,et al.  Infants' ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action , 2002, Cognition.

[56]  S. Baron-Cohen How to build a baby that can read minds: Cognitive mechanisms in mindreading. , 1994 .

[57]  G. Csibra,et al.  Goal attribution without agency cues: the perception of ‘pure reason’ in infancy , 1999, Cognition.

[58]  J. Mandler Perceptual and Conceptual Processes in Infancy , 2000 .

[59]  M. Hoffman,et al.  Developmental synthesis of affect and cognition and its implications for altruistic motivation. , 1975 .

[60]  Ayanna M. Howard,et al.  Effect of feedback from a socially interactive humanoid robot on reaching kinematics in children with and without cerebral palsy: A pilot study , 2018, Developmental neurorehabilitation.

[61]  Christoph Bartneck,et al.  To kill a mockingbird robot , 2007, 2007 2nd ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI).

[62]  F. Colmenares,et al.  Two and a half-year-old children are prosocial even when their partners are not. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[63]  V. Kuhlmeier,et al.  Infants’ Understanding of Object-Directed Action: An Interdisciplinary Synthesis , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[64]  Fabio Tesser,et al.  Interpretation of Emotional Body Language Displayed by a Humanoid Robot: A Case Study with Children , 2013, Int. J. Soc. Robotics.

[65]  Daniel L. Schwartz,et al.  Young Children's Understanding of Animacy and Entertainment Robots , 2006, Int. J. Humanoid Robotics.

[66]  Amanda L. Woodward,et al.  Infants’ developing understanding of the link between looker and object , 2003 .