Long-term repetition priming in spoken and written word production: evidence for a contribution of phonology to handwriting.

Although it is relatively well established that access to orthographic codes in production tasks is possible via an autonomous link between meaning and spelling (e.g., Rapp, Benzing, & Caramazza, 1997), the relative contribution of phonology to orthographic access remains unclear. Two experiments demonstrated persistent repetition priming in spoken and written single-word responses, respectively. Two further experiments showed priming from spoken to written responses and vice versa, which is interpreted as reflecting a role of phonology in constraining orthographic access. A final experiment showed priming from spoken onto written responses even when participants engaged in articulatory suppression during writing. Overall, the results support the view that access to orthography codes is accomplished via both the autonomous link between meaning and spelling and an indirect route via phonology.

[1]  D. Schacter Perceptual Representation Systems and Implicit Memory , 1990, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[2]  Derek Besner,et al.  Phonology, Lexical Access in Reading, and Articulatory Suppression: A Critical Review , 1987 .

[3]  G. Assal,et al.  Dissociations in aphasia: A case report , 1981, Brain and Language.

[4]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The role of phonological and orthographic information in lexical selection , 2003, Brain and Language.

[5]  D B DESAI,et al.  Traumatic aphasia. , 1947, The Antiseptic.

[6]  Jeremy J Tree,et al.  Sometimes faster, sometimes slower: associative and competitor priming in picture naming with young and elderly participants , 2003, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[7]  Gerard P. van Galen,et al.  Handwriting: Issues for a psychomotor theory ☆ , 1991 .

[8]  D. Allport,et al.  Components of the Mental Lexicon , 1981 .

[9]  J. H. Neely Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. , 1991 .

[10]  A. Beaton,et al.  Semantic Errors of Naming, Reading, Writing, and Drawing Following Left-Hemisphere Infarction , 1997 .

[11]  M. Charlton Pathologie du Language. , 1966 .

[12]  D. Bub,et al.  Evidence for lexicographic processing in a patient with preserved written over oral single word naming. , 1982, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[13]  S. Monsell,et al.  Inhibition of Spoken Word Production by Priming a Semantic Competitor , 1994 .

[14]  Christopher Barry,et al.  Age of Acquisition, Word Frequency, and the Locus of Repetition Priming of Picture Naming , 2001 .

[15]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The Modality-Specific Organization of Grammatical Categories: Evidence from Impaired Spoken and Written Sentence Production , 1997, Brain and Language.

[16]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Language perception and production , 1987 .

[17]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Basic processes in reading : visual word recognition , 1993 .

[18]  A. Caramazza,et al.  The Autonomy of Lexical Orthography , 1997 .

[19]  J. Bowers,et al.  Different perceptual codes support priming for words and pseudowords: was Morton right all along? , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[20]  R. Baayen,et al.  Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items , 2008 .

[21]  Christopher Barry,et al.  Naming the Snodgrass and Vanderwart Pictures: Effects of Age of Acquisition, Frequency, and Name Agreement , 1997 .

[22]  R. Peereman,et al.  Do Phonological Codes Constrain the Selection of Orthographic Codes in Written Picture Naming , 2001 .

[23]  C. J. McGrath,et al.  Effect of exchange rate return on volatility spill-over across trading regions , 2012 .

[24]  Elisabeth Dévière,et al.  Analyzing linguistic data: a practical introduction to statistics using R , 2009 .

[25]  S Monsell,et al.  The Locus of Repetition Priming of Spoken Word Production , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[26]  Leonard F. M. Scinto,et al.  Written language and psychological development , 1986 .

[27]  Glenn M. Kleiman,et al.  Speech recoding in reading , 1975 .

[28]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[29]  Matthew H. Davis,et al.  Mix, a program for pseudorandomization , 2006, Behavior research methods.

[30]  Rita Capasso,et al.  Semantic Errors as Neuropsychological Evidence for the Independence and the Interaction of Orthographic and Phonological Word Forms , 1997 .

[31]  K. Patterson,et al.  Speak and spell: Dissociations and word-class effects. , 1987 .

[32]  M. Coltheart,et al.  The cognitive neuropsychology of language , 1987 .

[33]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Word length and the structure of short-term memory , 1975 .

[34]  Alan S. Brown,et al.  Persistent repetition priming in picture naming and its dissociation from recognition memory. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[35]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.

[36]  Jeffrey S. Bowers,et al.  An investigation into the structure and acquisition of orthographic knowledge: Evidence from cross-script Kanji-Hiragana priming , 1998 .

[37]  A. Benton Contributions to clinical neuropsychology , 1969 .

[38]  A. Caramazza,et al.  The structure of graphemic representations , 1990, Cognition.

[39]  M. Damian,et al.  Impact of phonology on the generation of handwritten responses: Evidence from picture-word interference tasks , 2010, Memory & cognition.

[40]  William Badecker,et al.  Representational properties common to phonological and orthographic output systems , 1996 .

[41]  HighWire Press Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London , 1781, The London Medical Journal.

[42]  Michele Miozzo,et al.  Evidence for a cascade model of lexical access in speech production. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[43]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory , 1980 .

[44]  Jeffrey S. Bowers,et al.  Developing theories of priming with an eye on function , 2003 .

[45]  Alex Martin,et al.  Long-lasting cortical plasticity in the object naming system , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[46]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  NORMAL AND IMPAIRED SPELLING IN A CONNECTIONIST DUAL-ROUTE ARCHITECTURE , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[47]  M. Weinrich,et al.  Further Evidence of a Dissociation between Output Phonological and Orthographic Lexicons: A Case Study , 1997 .

[48]  Jeffrey S. Bowers,et al.  Rethinking Implicit Memory , 2002 .