Are accuracy discernment and sharing of COVID-19 misinformation associated with older age and lower neurocognitive functioning?
暂无分享,去创建一个
C. Neighbors | S. Woods | K. Podell | Jennifer L Thompson | A. Matchanova | L. Medina | I. Beltran-Najera | Christina Alex | Michelle A. Babicz
[1] S. Woods,et al. Preliminary Validity of a Telephone-Based Neuropsychological Battery in a Consecutive Series of Persons with HIV Disease Referred for Clinical Evaluation. , 2022, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.
[2] S. van der Linden. Misinformation: susceptibility, spread, and interventions to immunize the public , 2022, Nature Medicine.
[3] S. Woods,et al. How did individual differences in neurocognition and health literacy influence the initial uptake and use of health-related information about COVID-19? , 2020, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.
[4] David G. Rand,et al. Shifting attention to accuracy can reduce misinformation online , 2021, Nature.
[5] S. Lewandowsky,et al. COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy in the UK: the Oxford coronavirus explanations, attitudes, and narratives survey (Oceans) II , 2020, Psychological Medicine.
[6] S. Woods,et al. Latent Structure of a Brief Clinical Battery of Neuropsychological Tests Administered In-Home Via Telephone. , 2020, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.
[7] Jon Roozenbeek,et al. Susceptibility to misinformation about COVID-19 around the world , 2020, Royal Society Open Science.
[8] D. Jolley,et al. Pylons ablaze: Examining the role of 5G COVID‐19 conspiracy beliefs and support for violence , 2020, The British journal of social psychology.
[9] P. Delfabbro,et al. COVID-19-related conspiracy beliefs and their relationship with perceived stress and pre-existing conspiracy beliefs , 2020, Personality and Individual Differences.
[10] William J. Brady,et al. The MAD Model of Moral Contagion: The Role of Motivation, Attention, and Design in the Spread of Moralized Content Online , 2019, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.
[11] David G. Rand,et al. Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response , 2020, Nature Human Behaviour.
[12] David G. Rand,et al. Fighting COVID-19 Misinformation on Social Media: Experimental Evidence for a Scalable Accuracy-Nudge Intervention , 2020, Psychological science.
[13] Heidi Larson,et al. The pandemic of social media panic travels faster than the COVID-19 outbreak , 2020, Journal of travel medicine.
[14] Duncan Watts,et al. Evaluating the fake news problem at the scale of the information ecosystem , 2019, Science Advances.
[15] David G. Rand,et al. Fake news, fast and slow: Deliberation reduces belief in false (but not true) news headlines. , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[16] David G. Rand,et al. Self-reported willingness to share political news articles in online surveys correlates with actual sharing on Twitter , 2019, PloS one.
[17] Joshua B. Plotkin,et al. Information gerrymandering and undemocratic decisions , 2019, Nature.
[18] E. Field-Fote. Mediators and Moderators, Confounders and Covariates: Exploring the Variables That Illuminate or Obscure the "Active Ingredients" in Neurorehabilitation. , 2019, Journal of neurologic physical therapy : JNPT.
[19] David G. Rand,et al. Belief in fake news is associated with delusionality, dogmatism, religious fundamentalism, and reduced analytic thinking. , 2019, Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition.
[20] D. Lazer,et al. Fake news on Twitter during the 2016 U.S. presidential election , 2019, Science.
[21] Joshua A. Tucker,et al. Less than you think: Prevalence and predictors of fake news dissemination on Facebook , 2019, Science Advances.
[22] David G. Rand,et al. Belief in Fake News Is Associated with Delusionality, Dogmatism, Religious Fundamentalism, and Reduced Analytic Thinking , 2018, Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition.
[23] Kevin Arceneaux,et al. The “Need for Chaos” and Motivations to Share Hostile Political Rumors , 2018 .
[24] J. van Prooijen,et al. Increased conspiracy beliefs among ethnic and Muslim minorities , 2018, Applied Cognitive Psychology.
[25] David G. Rand,et al. Prior Exposure Increases Perceived Accuracy of Fake News , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[26] Sinan Aral,et al. The spread of true and false news online , 2018, Science.
[27] A. Roets,et al. The opposite effects of actual and self-perceived intelligence on racial prejudice , 2017 .
[28] Joshua A. Tucker,et al. Emotion shapes the diffusion of moralized content in social networks , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[29] M. Schmitter-Edgecombe,et al. Multiple Types of Memory and Everyday Functional Assessment in Older Adults , 2017, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.
[30] Ullrich K. H. Ecker,et al. The Role of Familiarity in Correcting Inaccurate Information , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[31] A. Berchtold,et al. Measuring internet use: comparisons of different assessments and with internet addiction , 2017 .
[32] J. van Prooijen,et al. Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories , 2016, Applied cognitive psychology.
[33] Alberto Acerbi. A Cultural Evolution Approach to Digital Media , 2016, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[34] Sundar Sarukkai. The age of post-truth politics , 2016 .
[35] G. Caldarelli,et al. The spreading of misinformation online , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[36] J. Hochschild,et al. Do Facts Matter? Information and Misinformation in American Politics , 2015 .
[37] Veronika Karnowski,et al. News Sharing in Social Media: A Review of Current Research on News Sharing Users, Content, and Networks , 2015 .
[38] Lada A. Adamic,et al. Exposure to ideologically diverse news and opinion on Facebook , 2015, Science.
[39] Jennie Q. Lou,et al. A systematic review of eHealth interventions to improve health literacy , 2016, AMIA.
[40] U. Lindenberger. Human cognitive aging: Corriger la fortune? , 2014, Science.
[41] Yunzhi Liu,et al. The Development and Validation of the Social Network Sites (SNSs) Usage Questionnaire , 2014, HCI.
[42] Gregory N. Mandel,et al. The Polarizing Impact of Science Literacy and Numeracy on Perceived Climate Change Risks , 2012 .
[43] Ullrich K. H. Ecker,et al. Misinformation and Its Correction , 2012, Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society.
[44] L. Jacoby,et al. The process-dissociation approach two decades later: Convergence, boundary conditions, and new directions , 2012, Memory & cognition.
[45] Stacey L. Sheridan,et al. Low Health Literacy and Health Outcomes: An Updated Systematic Review , 2011, Annals of Internal Medicine.
[46] R. Kumar,et al. Validation of a brief seven-item response bias-free geriatric depression scale. , 2011, The American journal of geriatric psychiatry : official journal of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry.
[47] Danah Boyd,et al. I tweet honestly, I tweet passionately: Twitter users, context collapse, and the imagined audience , 2011, New Media Soc..
[48] Stephan Lewandowsky,et al. Explicit warnings reduce but do not eliminate the continued influence of misinformation , 2010, Memory & cognition.
[49] E. Loftus,et al. Title Individual differences in false memory from misinformation : Cognitive factors Permalink , 2015 .
[50] N. Pachana,et al. Development and validation of a short form of the Geriatric Anxiety Inventory – the GAI-SF , 2010, International Psychogeriatrics.
[51] Ullrich K. H. Ecker,et al. Remembering perceptual features unequally bound in object and episodic tokens: Neural mechanisms and their electrophysiological correlates , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[52] D. Drane,et al. The oral trail making test: effects of age and concurrent validity. , 2010, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.
[53] D. Wechsler,et al. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV) , 2010 .
[54] E. Erdfelder,et al. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses , 2009, Behavior research methods.
[55] Ron Dumont,et al. Delis‐Kaplan Executive Function System , 2008 .
[56] Siamak Noorbaloochi,et al. Validation of Screening Questions for Limited Health Literacy in a Large VA Outpatient Population , 2008, Journal of General Internal Medicine.
[57] Cameron D. Norman,et al. eHEALS: The eHealth Literacy Scale , 2006, Journal of medical Internet research.
[58] Elizabeth Clea Warburton,et al. Associations and dissociations in recognition memory systems , 2006, Handbook of Binding and Memory.
[59] Roger Eeckels,et al. Data Cleaning: Detecting, Diagnosing, and Editing Data Abnormalities , 2005, PLoS medicine.
[60] A. Tröster,et al. Action (verb) fluency: Test–retest reliability, normative standards, and construct validity , 2005, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.
[61] Judith Donath,et al. Public Displays of Connection , 2004 .
[62] B. Rimer,et al. General Performance on a Numeracy Scale among Highly Educated Samples , 2001, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.
[63] A. Tröster,et al. Action (verb naming) fluency as an executive function measure: convergent and divergent evidence of validity , 1999, Neuropsychologia.
[64] Michael H. Goldhaber,et al. The Attention Economy and the Net , 1997, First Monday.
[65] Ted Goertzel,et al. Belief in Conspiracy Theories , 1994 .
[66] J. Ricker,et al. Analysis of an Oral Paradigm for the Trail Making Test , 1994, Assessment.
[67] Marcia K. Johnson,et al. Source monitoring. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.
[68] Morton B. Brown,et al. Robust Tests for the Equality of Variances , 1974 .