Are accuracy discernment and sharing of COVID-19 misinformation associated with older age and lower neurocognitive functioning?

[1]  S. Woods,et al.  Preliminary Validity of a Telephone-Based Neuropsychological Battery in a Consecutive Series of Persons with HIV Disease Referred for Clinical Evaluation. , 2022, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[2]  S. van der Linden Misinformation: susceptibility, spread, and interventions to immunize the public , 2022, Nature Medicine.

[3]  S. Woods,et al.  How did individual differences in neurocognition and health literacy influence the initial uptake and use of health-related information about COVID-19? , 2020, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[4]  David G. Rand,et al.  Shifting attention to accuracy can reduce misinformation online , 2021, Nature.

[5]  S. Lewandowsky,et al.  COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy in the UK: the Oxford coronavirus explanations, attitudes, and narratives survey (Oceans) II , 2020, Psychological Medicine.

[6]  S. Woods,et al.  Latent Structure of a Brief Clinical Battery of Neuropsychological Tests Administered In-Home Via Telephone. , 2020, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[7]  Jon Roozenbeek,et al.  Susceptibility to misinformation about COVID-19 around the world , 2020, Royal Society Open Science.

[8]  D. Jolley,et al.  Pylons ablaze: Examining the role of 5G COVID‐19 conspiracy beliefs and support for violence , 2020, The British journal of social psychology.

[9]  P. Delfabbro,et al.  COVID-19-related conspiracy beliefs and their relationship with perceived stress and pre-existing conspiracy beliefs , 2020, Personality and Individual Differences.

[10]  William J. Brady,et al.  The MAD Model of Moral Contagion: The Role of Motivation, Attention, and Design in the Spread of Moralized Content Online , 2019, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[11]  David G. Rand,et al.  Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response , 2020, Nature Human Behaviour.

[12]  David G. Rand,et al.  Fighting COVID-19 Misinformation on Social Media: Experimental Evidence for a Scalable Accuracy-Nudge Intervention , 2020, Psychological science.

[13]  Heidi Larson,et al.  The pandemic of social media panic travels faster than the COVID-19 outbreak , 2020, Journal of travel medicine.

[14]  Duncan Watts,et al.  Evaluating the fake news problem at the scale of the information ecosystem , 2019, Science Advances.

[15]  David G. Rand,et al.  Fake news, fast and slow: Deliberation reduces belief in false (but not true) news headlines. , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[16]  David G. Rand,et al.  Self-reported willingness to share political news articles in online surveys correlates with actual sharing on Twitter , 2019, PloS one.

[17]  Joshua B. Plotkin,et al.  Information gerrymandering and undemocratic decisions , 2019, Nature.

[18]  E. Field-Fote Mediators and Moderators, Confounders and Covariates: Exploring the Variables That Illuminate or Obscure the "Active Ingredients" in Neurorehabilitation. , 2019, Journal of neurologic physical therapy : JNPT.

[19]  David G. Rand,et al.  Belief in fake news is associated with delusionality, dogmatism, religious fundamentalism, and reduced analytic thinking. , 2019, Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition.

[20]  D. Lazer,et al.  Fake news on Twitter during the 2016 U.S. presidential election , 2019, Science.

[21]  Joshua A. Tucker,et al.  Less than you think: Prevalence and predictors of fake news dissemination on Facebook , 2019, Science Advances.

[22]  David G. Rand,et al.  Belief in Fake News Is Associated with Delusionality, Dogmatism, Religious Fundamentalism, and Reduced Analytic Thinking , 2018, Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition.

[23]  Kevin Arceneaux,et al.  The “Need for Chaos” and Motivations to Share Hostile Political Rumors , 2018 .

[24]  J. van Prooijen,et al.  Increased conspiracy beliefs among ethnic and Muslim minorities , 2018, Applied Cognitive Psychology.

[25]  David G. Rand,et al.  Prior Exposure Increases Perceived Accuracy of Fake News , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[26]  Sinan Aral,et al.  The spread of true and false news online , 2018, Science.

[27]  A. Roets,et al.  The opposite effects of actual and self-perceived intelligence on racial prejudice , 2017 .

[28]  Joshua A. Tucker,et al.  Emotion shapes the diffusion of moralized content in social networks , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[29]  M. Schmitter-Edgecombe,et al.  Multiple Types of Memory and Everyday Functional Assessment in Older Adults , 2017, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[30]  Ullrich K. H. Ecker,et al.  The Role of Familiarity in Correcting Inaccurate Information , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[31]  A. Berchtold,et al.  Measuring internet use: comparisons of different assessments and with internet addiction , 2017 .

[32]  J. van Prooijen,et al.  Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories , 2016, Applied cognitive psychology.

[33]  Alberto Acerbi A Cultural Evolution Approach to Digital Media , 2016, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[34]  Sundar Sarukkai The age of post-truth politics , 2016 .

[35]  G. Caldarelli,et al.  The spreading of misinformation online , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[36]  J. Hochschild,et al.  Do Facts Matter? Information and Misinformation in American Politics , 2015 .

[37]  Veronika Karnowski,et al.  News Sharing in Social Media: A Review of Current Research on News Sharing Users, Content, and Networks , 2015 .

[38]  Lada A. Adamic,et al.  Exposure to ideologically diverse news and opinion on Facebook , 2015, Science.

[39]  Jennie Q. Lou,et al.  A systematic review of eHealth interventions to improve health literacy , 2016, AMIA.

[40]  U. Lindenberger Human cognitive aging: Corriger la fortune? , 2014, Science.

[41]  Yunzhi Liu,et al.  The Development and Validation of the Social Network Sites (SNSs) Usage Questionnaire , 2014, HCI.

[42]  Gregory N. Mandel,et al.  The Polarizing Impact of Science Literacy and Numeracy on Perceived Climate Change Risks , 2012 .

[43]  Ullrich K. H. Ecker,et al.  Misinformation and Its Correction , 2012, Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society.

[44]  L. Jacoby,et al.  The process-dissociation approach two decades later: Convergence, boundary conditions, and new directions , 2012, Memory & cognition.

[45]  Stacey L. Sheridan,et al.  Low Health Literacy and Health Outcomes: An Updated Systematic Review , 2011, Annals of Internal Medicine.

[46]  R. Kumar,et al.  Validation of a brief seven-item response bias-free geriatric depression scale. , 2011, The American journal of geriatric psychiatry : official journal of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry.

[47]  Danah Boyd,et al.  I tweet honestly, I tweet passionately: Twitter users, context collapse, and the imagined audience , 2011, New Media Soc..

[48]  Stephan Lewandowsky,et al.  Explicit warnings reduce but do not eliminate the continued influence of misinformation , 2010, Memory & cognition.

[49]  E. Loftus,et al.  Title Individual differences in false memory from misinformation : Cognitive factors Permalink , 2015 .

[50]  N. Pachana,et al.  Development and validation of a short form of the Geriatric Anxiety Inventory – the GAI-SF , 2010, International Psychogeriatrics.

[51]  Ullrich K. H. Ecker,et al.  Remembering perceptual features unequally bound in object and episodic tokens: Neural mechanisms and their electrophysiological correlates , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[52]  D. Drane,et al.  The oral trail making test: effects of age and concurrent validity. , 2010, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[53]  D. Wechsler,et al.  Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV) , 2010 .

[54]  E. Erdfelder,et al.  Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses , 2009, Behavior research methods.

[55]  Ron Dumont,et al.  Delis‐Kaplan Executive Function System , 2008 .

[56]  Siamak Noorbaloochi,et al.  Validation of Screening Questions for Limited Health Literacy in a Large VA Outpatient Population , 2008, Journal of General Internal Medicine.

[57]  Cameron D. Norman,et al.  eHEALS: The eHealth Literacy Scale , 2006, Journal of medical Internet research.

[58]  Elizabeth Clea Warburton,et al.  Associations and dissociations in recognition memory systems , 2006, Handbook of Binding and Memory.

[59]  Roger Eeckels,et al.  Data Cleaning: Detecting, Diagnosing, and Editing Data Abnormalities , 2005, PLoS medicine.

[60]  A. Tröster,et al.  Action (verb) fluency: Test–retest reliability, normative standards, and construct validity , 2005, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[61]  Judith Donath,et al.  Public Displays of Connection , 2004 .

[62]  B. Rimer,et al.  General Performance on a Numeracy Scale among Highly Educated Samples , 2001, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[63]  A. Tröster,et al.  Action (verb naming) fluency as an executive function measure: convergent and divergent evidence of validity , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[64]  Michael H. Goldhaber,et al.  The Attention Economy and the Net , 1997, First Monday.

[65]  Ted Goertzel,et al.  Belief in Conspiracy Theories , 1994 .

[66]  J. Ricker,et al.  Analysis of an Oral Paradigm for the Trail Making Test , 1994, Assessment.

[67]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  Source monitoring. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[68]  Morton B. Brown,et al.  Robust Tests for the Equality of Variances , 1974 .