Effect of tactile stimulus frequency on time perception: the role of working memory

In most models of interval timing, there is a central clock, which is considered to be highly protected from the effects of external stimuli. However, many studies have reported such effects and different theories are proposed to explain the observations. These include the effect of arousal, attention sharing, memory load and information processing on central clock as well as change in the speed of the pacemaker. In this study, we used regular vibro-tactile stimuli with different frequencies in a “duration reproduction task” to investigate the effect of stimulus content on interval timing. Results showed that subjects overestimated the duration as a function of test stimulus frequency. A significant correlation between increasing the test frequency and overestimation of subjective time was observed. We further investigated the effect of blank and filled gaps with various durations on time estimation. Analysis revealed that regardless of gap duration, subjective time increased in the filled gap condition, compared to the blank gap. This effect was independent from contextual stimuli and correlated to the mean number of stimuli during the temporal interval rather than rate of stimulus presentation.

[1]  D. Zakay Time Estimation Methods—Do They Influence Prospective Duration Estimates? , 1993, Perception.

[2]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Scalar expectancy theory and peak-interval timing in humans. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[3]  Scott W. Brown,et al.  Attentional processes in time perception: effects of mental workload and event structure. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  W. Meck Attentional Bias between Modalities: Effect on the Internal Clock, Memory, and Decision Stages Used in Animal Time Discrimination a , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[5]  M. Frankenhaeuser,et al.  Estimation of time , 1959 .

[6]  Richard A. Block,et al.  Remembered Duration: Evidence for a Contextual-Change Hypothesis , 2005 .

[7]  J Predebon The relationship between the number of presented stimuli and prospective duration estimates: The effect of concurrent task activity , 1996, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[8]  C. Douglas Creelman,et al.  Human Discrimination of Auditory Duration , 1962 .

[9]  Rosenberg,et al.  Estimation of Time , 1922 .

[10]  Claudette Fortin,et al.  Timing during interruptions in timing. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  P. Tse,et al.  Attention and the subjective expansion of time , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.

[12]  R. Ornstein,et al.  The experience of time , 1986 .

[13]  M. Treisman,et al.  The Internal Clock: Evidence for a Temporal Oscillator Underlying Time Perception with Some Estimates of its Characteristic Frequency , 1990, Perception.

[14]  R M Church,et al.  Scalar Timing in Memory , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[15]  Ewart A. C. Thomas,et al.  Cognitive processing and time perception , 1975 .

[16]  W H Meck,et al.  Timing for the absence of a stimulus: the gap paradigm reversed. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[17]  Michel Treisman,et al.  Time perception and the internal clock: Effects of visual flicker on the temporal oscillator , 1992 .

[18]  Control of attention in the processing of temporal and spatial information in complex visual patterns. , 1977, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  D. Buonomano,et al.  The neural basis of temporal processing. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[20]  J H Wearden,et al.  Speeding up an internal clock in humans? Effects of click trains on subjective duration. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[21]  W. N. Schoenfeld The Theory of reinforcement schedules , 1970 .

[22]  S. Grondin,et al.  From physical time to the first and second moments of psychological time. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[23]  R. Block,et al.  Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: an executive-control perspective. , 2004, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[24]  J. Gibbon Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. , 1977 .

[25]  C Fortin,et al.  Expecting a break in time estimation: attentional time-sharing without concurrent processing. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  R. Block,et al.  Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: A meta-analytic review , 1997, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[27]  Sébastien Tremblay,et al.  Break expectancy in duration discrimination. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  Warren H. Meck,et al.  Symmetrical and asymmetrical sources of variance in temporal generalization , 1991 .

[29]  Simon Grondin,et al.  Duration discrimination of empty and filled intervals marked by auditory and visual signals , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[30]  M. Treisman Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval. Implications for a model of the "internal clock". , 1963, Psychological monographs.

[31]  Yehoshua Tsal,et al.  Awareness of attention allocation and time estimation accuracy , 1989 .

[32]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Timing and time perception. , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[33]  R. M. Mulligan,et al.  Temporal experience as a function of organization in memory , 1979 .

[34]  R. E. Hicks,et al.  Prospective and retrospective judgments of time as a function of amount of information processed. , 1976, The American journal of psychology.

[35]  Catalin V Buhusi,et al.  Temporal integration as a function of signal and gap intensity in rats (Rattus norvegicus) and pigeons (Columba livia). , 2002, Journal of comparative psychology.

[36]  P. Tse,et al.  Time and the Brain: How Subjective Time Relates to Neural Time , 2005 .

[37]  F. Macar,et al.  Controlled attention sharing influences time estimation , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[38]  Dan Zakay,et al.  Gating or switching? Gating is a better model of prospective timing (a response to ‘switching or gating?’ by Lejeune) 1 Originally published in Vol. 50, issue 1, pages 1–17; PII of the original publication: S0376-6357(00)00086-3. 1 , 2000, Behavioural Processes.

[39]  P. Fraisse Perception and estimation of time. , 1984, Annual review of psychology.

[40]  R. Ivry,et al.  The neural representation of time , 2004, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[41]  P. Killeen,et al.  A behavioral theory of timing. , 1988, Psychological review.

[42]  C. Buhusi,et al.  Differential effects of methamphetamine and haloperidol on the control of an internal clock. , 2002, Behavioral neuroscience.

[43]  Justin A. Harris,et al.  The Topography of Tactile Working Memory , 2001, Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  Catalin V. Buhusi,et al.  What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[45]  R. Church,et al.  Representation of time , 1990, Cognition.