The development of an appreciation of specific linkages between linguistic and conceptual organization

Abstract Evidence from infants and toddlers, and from preschool children learning either English, French, or Spanish as their first language are summarized to reveal the emergence of specific linkages between linguistic and conceptual development. The data suggest that infants begin the process of word learning with a general expectation that words (independent of the linguistic form) refer to objects and object categories. This initial, rudimentary linkage gives way to more specific pairings between particular linguistic forms (e.g., nouns vs. adjectives) and particular types of meaning (e.g., object categories vs. properties of objects). These more specific linkages may depend upon language experience.

[1]  W. Merriman,et al.  The mutual exclusivity bias in children's word learning. , 1989, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[2]  Steven Pinker,et al.  Language learnability and language development , 1985 .

[3]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  How Two‐ and Four‐Year‐Old Children Interpret Adjectives and Count Nouns , 1993 .

[4]  J. Mandler,et al.  Taxonomies and triads: Conceptual organization in one- to two-year-olds , 1989, Cognitive Psychology.

[5]  Barbara A. Shepperson,et al.  Establishing New Subcategories: The Role of Category Labels and Existing Knowledge , 1991 .

[6]  J. Mandler How to build a baby: On the development of an accessible representational system☆ , 1988 .

[7]  P. Gordon Evaluating the semantic categories hypothesis: The case of the count/mass distinction , 1985, Cognition.

[8]  Willard Van Orman Quine,et al.  Word and Object , 1960 .

[9]  Lucile Y. Paden The effects of variations of auditory stimulation (music) and interspersed stimulus procedures on visual attending behavior in infants. , 1974, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[10]  C. Mervis,et al.  Role of adult input in young children's category evolution. I. An observational study , 1988, Journal of Child Language.

[11]  John McShane,et al.  Learning to Talk , 1980 .

[12]  T. Seiler,et al.  Concept development and the development of word meaning , 1983 .

[13]  E. Markman,et al.  Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words , 1988, Cognitive Psychology.

[14]  Leslie J. Francis,et al.  Separating Sheep from Goats , 2012, British medical journal.

[15]  C. N. Cofer Origins of the journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior , 1978 .

[16]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  Linguistic biases and the establishment of conceptual hierarchies: Evidence from preschool children , 1990 .

[17]  H. Benedict,et al.  Early lexical development: comprehension and production , 1979, Journal of Child Language.

[18]  C. L. Baker,et al.  The Logical problem of language acquisition , 1984 .

[19]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Establishing word-object relations: a first step. , 1989, Child development.

[20]  A. Leslie,et al.  Do six-month-old infants perceive causality? , 1987, Cognition.

[21]  E. Markman Categorization and naming in children , 1989 .

[22]  Susan Carey,et al.  Acquiring a Single New Word , 1978 .

[23]  J. M. Anglin,et al.  Word, object, and conceptual development , 1977 .

[24]  Kenneth Roberts,et al.  Linguistic versus attentional influences on nonlinguistic categorization in 15-month-old infants , 1991 .

[25]  J. Reznick,et al.  The Development of Contextual Categories , 1987 .

[26]  U. Neisser Concepts and Conceptual Development: Ecological and Intellectual Factors in Categorization , 1989 .

[27]  T. Au,et al.  Children's use of information in word learning , 1990, Journal of Child Language.

[28]  Roger S. Brown,et al.  Linguistic determinism and the part of speech. , 1957, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[29]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  The importance of shape in early lexical learning , 1988 .

[30]  L. Cohen,et al.  Infant perception of correlations among attributes. , 1983, Child development.

[31]  N. Goodman Fact, Fiction, and Forecast , 1955 .

[32]  J. Reznick,et al.  Early lexical acquisition: rate, content, and the vocabulary spurt , 1990, Journal of Child Language.

[33]  H. Gleitman,et al.  Linguistics: The Cambridge Survey: Where learning begins: initial representations for language learning , 1988 .

[34]  Mendelson Mj,et al.  The relation between audition and vision in the human newborn. , 1976 .

[35]  Patricia Howlin,et al.  Origins of cognitive skills , 1986 .

[36]  Letitia R. Naigles,et al.  Children use syntax to learn verb meanings , 1990, Journal of Child Language.

[37]  A. Gopnik,et al.  The Development of Categorization in the Second Year and Its Relation to Other Cognitive and Linguistic Developments. , 1987 .

[38]  M. Mendelson,et al.  The relation between audition and vision in the human newborn. , 1976, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[39]  E. Markman,et al.  Children's sensitivity to constraints on word meaning: Taxonomic versus thematic relations , 1984, Cognitive Psychology.

[40]  D. Geoffrey Hall,et al.  Acquiring Proper Nouns for Familiar and Unfamiliar Animate Objects: Two-Year-Olds' Word-learning Biases , 1991 .

[41]  Maureen A. Callanan,et al.  How Parents Label Objects for Young Children: The Role of Input in the Acquisition of Category Hierarchies. , 1985 .

[42]  Sandra R. Waxman Convergences between semantic and conceptual organization in the preschool years , 1991 .

[43]  C. Mervis Child-basic object categories and early lexical development. , 1987 .

[44]  E. Markman,et al.  Implicit contrast in adjectives vs. nouns: implications for word-learning in preschoolers , 1985, Journal of Child Language.

[45]  J. Mandler,et al.  Separating the sheep from the goats: Differentiating global categories , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[46]  P. Kaplan,et al.  Cross-modal facilitation of infant visual fixation: Temporal and intensity effects☆ , 1991 .

[47]  S. Carey The child as word learner , 1978 .

[48]  E. Dromi Early Lexical Development , 1987 .

[49]  P. A. Self Control of infant visual attending by auditory and interspersed stimulation. , 1974, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[50]  S. Prasada Learning names for solid substances: Quantifying solid entities in terms of portions ☆ , 1993 .

[51]  M. Tomasello The Role of Joint Attentional Processes in Early Language Development. , 1988 .

[52]  S. Gelman,et al.  Incorporating new words into the lexicon: preliminary evidence for language hierarchies in two-year-old children. , 1989, Child development.

[53]  S. Waxman,et al.  The development of a linkage between count nouns and object categories: evidence from fifteen- to twenty-one-month-old infants. , 1993, Child development.

[54]  C. Malatesta,et al.  The development of emotion expression during the first two years of life. , 1989, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[55]  C. F. Hockett,et al.  Words and Things. , 1959 .

[56]  Alison Gopnik,et al.  Three types of early word: the emergence of social words, names and cognitive-relational words in the one-word stage and their relation to cognitive development , 1988 .

[57]  H. Ruff Components of attention during infants' manipulative exploration. , 1986, Child development.

[58]  Knowledge-Derived Categorization in Young Children , 1983 .

[59]  Virginia Valian Syntactic categories in the speech of young children. , 1986 .

[60]  Jay L. Garfield,et al.  Review: John Macnamara, A Border Dispute. The Place of Logic in Psychology , 1988 .

[61]  David K. Dickinson Learning names for materials: Factors constraining and limiting hypotheses about word meaning ☆ , 1988 .

[62]  K. Nelson,et al.  The Conceptual Basis for Language , 1983 .

[63]  S. Gelman,et al.  How two-year-old children interpret proper and common names for unfamiliar objects. , 1984, Child development.

[64]  Roger W. Brown,et al.  Words and Things. , 1959 .

[65]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  Relations among Word Meanings in Early Lexical Development. , 1992 .

[66]  L B Cohen,et al.  Developmental change in infants' perception of correlations among attributes. , 1986, Child development.

[67]  L. J. Friedman Child Development , 1971, Nature.

[68]  D. H. Wheeler,et al.  The early growth of logic in the child : classification and seriation , 1965 .

[69]  A. Fernald Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. , 1992 .

[70]  Katherine Nelson,et al.  Constraints on word learning , 1988 .

[71]  L. M. Mcpherson,et al.  A little goes a long way: evidence for a perceptual basis of learning for the noun categories COUNT and Mass , 1991, Journal of Child Language.

[72]  L. Frank The Society for Research in Child Development , 1935 .

[73]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  Preschoolers' use of superordinate relations in classification and language , 1986 .

[74]  Gonzalo E. Reyes,et al.  The Logical Foundations of Cognition , 1994 .

[75]  S. Waxman,et al.  Nouns mark category relations: toddlers' and preschoolers' word-learning biases. , 1990, Child development.

[76]  L. Gleitman,et al.  Language and Experience: Evidence from the Blind Child , 1988 .

[77]  R. Gelman,et al.  The development of communication skills: modifications in the speech of young children as a function of listener. , 1973, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[78]  Kelly L. Madole,et al.  Infants' object examining: Habituation and categorization ☆ ☆☆ , 1991 .

[79]  E. Spelke,et al.  Ontological categories guide young children's inductions of word meaning: Object terms and substance terms , 1991, Cognition.

[80]  Sandra R. Waxman,et al.  Assumptions about Word Meaning: Individuation and Basic‐Level Kinds , 1993 .

[81]  William M. Smith,et al.  A Study of Thinking , 1956 .

[82]  Janellen Huttenlocher,et al.  The semantic organization of some simple nouns and verbs , 1979 .

[83]  C. Mervis,et al.  Role of adult input in young children's category evolution: II an experimental study , 1988, Journal of Child Language.

[84]  Leonard Talmy,et al.  (1) Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms; and , 1987 .

[85]  K. Nelson,et al.  Structure and strategy in learning to talk. , 1973 .

[86]  D. E. Breedlove,et al.  General Principles of Classification and Nomenclature in Folk Biology , 1973 .

[87]  P. Bloom Syntactic distinctions in child language , 1990, Journal of Child Language.

[88]  G. Ross Categorization in 1- to 2-yr-olds. , 1980 .

[89]  Susan A. Gelman,et al.  Adjectives and Nouns: Children's Strategies for Learning New Words. , 1988 .

[90]  C. Mervis,et al.  Order of acquisition of subordinate-, basic-, and superordinate-level categories. , 1982 .

[91]  L. Vygotsky,et al.  Thought and Language , 1963 .

[92]  Laura A. Petitto,et al.  On the autonomy of language and gesture: Evidence from the acquisition of personal pronouns in American sign language , 1987, Cognition.

[93]  J. Mandler,et al.  The cradle of categorization: Is the basic level basic? , 1988 .

[94]  R. Dixon Where Have all the Adjectives Gone , 1977 .

[95]  Eve V. Clark,et al.  The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acquisition. , 1987 .

[96]  L. Cosmides,et al.  The Adapted mind : evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture , 1992 .

[97]  John Macnamara,et al.  What's in a name? A study of how children learn common and proper names. , 1974 .

[98]  Dedre Gentner,et al.  Why Nouns Are Learned before Verbs: Linguistic Relativity Versus Natural Partitioning. Technical Report No. 257. , 1982 .

[99]  Leslie M. Bailey,et al.  Young children and adults use lexical principles to learn New Nouns , 1992 .

[100]  M. Kilwein,et al.  Basic objects in natural categories revisited : a replication with sighted and blind college students / , 1993 .

[101]  Alison Gopnik,et al.  Do linguistic differences lead to cognitive differences? A cross-linguistic study of semantic and cognitive development , 1990 .

[102]  Barbara A. Younger,et al.  Developmental change in infants' perception of correlations among attributes. , 1986, Child development.