What’s an internal clock for? From temporal information processing to temporal processing of information

The existence of an internal clock and its involvement in information processing has been investigated in humans using the experimental protocol of Treisman et al. (1990) [Treisman et al., 1990. Perception, 19, 705-743]. In this protocol, a periodical stimulation, which is assumed to drive an internal clock, is delivered during a reaction time (RT) task. The accelerating or slowing down effects of the periodical stimulation, according to its frequency, allowed an estimate to be made of a simple harmonic of the frequency of the internal clock. The estimate was close to 21 Hz. In the framework of the serial model of information processing, the present work investigates the involvement of the internal clock in the transmission of information between processing stages during RT. The data tend to support the idea that the internal clock allows the transfer of information from one stage to the next one at definite moments only, periodically distributed in time. According to our results, and recent data from the literature on electric cortical oscillations, we propose a model of an internal clock sending periodic inhibition, which would permit an increased signal/noise ratio in the processing and the transmission of information in the central nervous system.

[1]  S Kornblum,et al.  The way irrelevant dimensions are processed depends on what they overlap with: The case of Stroop- and Simon-like stimuli , 1994, Psychological research.

[2]  A. Kristofferson Quantal and Deterministic Timing in Human Duration Discrimination , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[3]  E. Fetz,et al.  Synchronization of neurons during local field potential oscillations in sensorimotor cortex of awake monkeys. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  William A. MacKay,et al.  Field potential oscillatory bursts in parietal cortex before and during reach , 1995, Brain Research.

[5]  F. Macar,et al.  Effects of attention manipulation on judgments of duration and of intensity in the visual modality , 1997, Memory & cognition.

[6]  G. P. van Galen,et al.  How continuous are discrete notions of human information processing , 1995 .

[7]  R. Traub,et al.  Neuronal networks for induced ‘40 Hz’ rhythms , 1996, Trends in Neurosciences.

[8]  D. Zakay,et al.  On Prospective Time Estimation, Temporal Relevance and Temporal Uncertainty , 1992 .

[9]  S. Sternberg,et al.  The meaning of additive reaction-time effects: tests of three alternatives , 1993 .

[10]  R M Church,et al.  Properties of the Internal Clock a , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[11]  J. Seal,et al.  Finger Pairings in Two-Choice Reaction Time Taskscolon: Does the Between-Hands Advantage Reflect Response Preparation? , 1995, Journal of motor behavior.

[12]  G Mulder,et al.  Selective response activation can begin before stimulus recognition is complete: a psychophysiological and error analysis of continuous flow. , 1990, Acta psychologica.

[13]  C. Marsden,et al.  Frequency peaks of tremor, muscle vibration and electromyographic activity at 10 Hz, 20 Hz and 40 Hz during human finger muscle contraction may reflect rhythmicities of central neural firing , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Temporal Oscillations in Human Perception , 1993 .

[15]  D Purves,et al.  Temporal events in cyclopean vision. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  Alexa Riehle,et al.  Preparation for Action: one of the Key Functions of Motor Cortex , 2004 .

[17]  P. Bertelson,et al.  The time-course of preparation: Confirmatory results with visual and auditory warning signals , 1969 .

[18]  J H Wearden,et al.  Speeding up an internal clock in humans? Effects of click trains on subjective duration. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[19]  B. Conway,et al.  Synchronization between motor cortex and spinal motoneuronal pool during the performance of a maintained motor task in man. , 1995, The Journal of physiology.

[20]  A Faulkner,et al.  On the Relation between Time Perception and the Timing of Motor Action: Evidence for a Temporal Oscillator Controlling the Timing of Movement , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[21]  J. Lisman Bursts as a unit of neural information: making unreliable synapses reliable , 1997, Trends in Neurosciences.

[22]  S. Dornič,et al.  Attention and performance V , 1976 .

[23]  Michel Treisman,et al.  Time perception and the internal clock: Effects of visual flicker on the temporal oscillator , 1992 .

[24]  Christa Neuper,et al.  40-Hz oscillations during motor behavior in man , 1993, Neuroscience Letters.

[25]  M. Treisman,et al.  The Internal Clock: Electroencephalographic Evidence for Oscillatory Processes Underlying Time Perception , 1994, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[26]  M Akamatsu,et al.  Preparatory inhibition of cortico-spinal excitability: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study in man. , 1997, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[27]  C. Eriksen,et al.  An electromyographic examination of response competition , 1985 .

[28]  C T White,et al.  Periodicity within Reaction Time Distributions and Electromyograms , 1968, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[29]  J. Bouyer,et al.  Fast fronto-parietal rhythms during combined focused attentive behaviour and immobility in cat: cortical and thalamic localizations. , 1981, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[30]  J R Simon,et al.  Processing auditory information: interference from an irrelevant cue. , 1969, The Journal of applied psychology.

[31]  W. Singer,et al.  Visuomotor integration is associated with zero time-lag synchronization among cortical areas , 1997, Nature.

[32]  Françoise Macar,et al.  Multiple approaches to investigate the existence of an internal clock using attentional resources , 1999, Behavioural Processes.

[33]  J. Takahashi,et al.  Calcium modulates circadian variation in cAMP-stimulated melatonin in chick pineal cells , 1996, Brain Research.

[34]  B. Burle,et al.  Further argument for the existence of a pacemaker in the human information processing system. , 1997, Acta psychologica.

[35]  J. Requin,et al.  HUMAN REFLEXOLOGY AND MOTOR PREPARATION , 1981, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[36]  G. Buzsáki,et al.  Gamma Oscillations in the Entorhinal Cortex of the Freely Behaving Rat , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  W. Mackay,et al.  EEG rhythms of the sensorimotor region during hand movements. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[38]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Discrete and continuous models of human information processing: theoretical distinctions and empirical results. , 1988, Acta psychologica.

[39]  M. Treisman,et al.  The Internal Clock: Evidence for a Temporal Oscillator Underlying Time Perception with Some Estimates of its Characteristic Frequency , 1990, Perception.

[40]  H. Clamann,et al.  Elsevier/North-Holland Biomedical Press COMPARISON OF THE RECRUITMENT AND DISCHARGE PROPERTIES OF MOTOR UNITS IN H U M A N BRACHIAL BICEPS AND A D D U C T O R POLLICIS D U R I N G ISOMETRIC CONTRACTIONS , 2018 .

[41]  R. Freeman,et al.  Oscillatory discharge in the visual system: does it have a functional role? , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[43]  V. Bringuier,et al.  Synaptic origin and stimulus dependency of neuronal oscillatory activity in the primary visual cortex of the cat. , 1997, The Journal of physiology.

[44]  H. Quastler Information theory in psychology , 1955 .

[45]  W. Singer,et al.  Stimulus-specific neuronal oscillations in orientation columns of cat visual cortex. , 1989, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[46]  C. Koch,et al.  Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness , 1990 .

[47]  E. Fetz,et al.  Oscillatory activity in sensorimotor cortex of awake monkeys: synchronization of local field potentials and relation to behavior. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  R. Kretchmar Exercise and Sport Science , 1989 .

[49]  E. Olivier,et al.  Coherent oscillations in monkey motor cortex and hand muscle EMG show task‐dependent modulation , 1997, The Journal of physiology.

[50]  Saul Sternberg,et al.  The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method , 1969 .

[51]  R. Traub,et al.  Synchronized oscillations in interneuron networks driven by metabotropic glutamate receptor activation , 1995, Nature.

[52]  K. Reinikainen,et al.  Selective attention enhances the auditory 40-Hz transient response in humans , 1993, Nature.

[53]  R. Llinás,et al.  Coherent 40-Hz oscillation characterizes dream state in humans. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[54]  R. Llinás,et al.  In vitro neurons in mammalian cortical layer 4 exhibit intrinsic oscillatory activity in the 10- to 50-Hz frequency range. , 1991, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[55]  Françoise Macar,et al.  Time, action, and cognition : towards bridging the gap , 1992 .

[56]  B. J. Winer Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1992 .

[57]  A. Sanders Issues and trends in the debate on discrete vs. continuous processing of information , 1990 .

[58]  J. Brožek Attention and Performance II. , 1971 .

[59]  K. Seki,et al.  Firing rate modulation of human motor units in different muscles during isometric contraction with various forces , 1996, Brain Research.

[60]  J. M. Stroud THE FINE STRUCTURE OF PSYCHOLOGICAL TIME , 1967 .