Timing matters: sonar call groups facilitate target localization in bats

To successfully negotiate a cluttered environment, an echolocating bat must control the timing of motor behaviors in response to dynamic sensory information. Here we detail the big brown bat's adaptive temporal control over sonar call production for tracking prey, moving predictably or unpredictably, under different experimental conditions. We studied the adaptive control of vocal-motor behaviors in free-flying big brown bats, Eptesicus fuscus, as they captured tethered and free-flying insects, in open and cluttered environments. We also studied adaptive sonar behavior in bats trained to track moving targets from a resting position. In each of these experiments, bats adjusted the features of their calls to separate target and clutter. Under many task conditions, flying bats produced prominent sonar sound groups identified as clusters of echolocation pulses with relatively stable intervals, surrounded by longer pulse intervals. In experiments where bats tracked approaching targets from a resting position, bats also produced sonar sound groups, and the prevalence of these sonar sound groups increased when motion of the target was unpredictable. We hypothesize that sonar sound groups produced during flight, and the sonar call doublets produced by a bat tracking a target from a resting position, help the animal resolve dynamic target location and represent the echo scene in greater detail. Collectively, our data reveal adaptive temporal control over sonar call production that allows the bat to negotiate a complex and dynamic environment.

[1]  H. Schnitzler,et al.  Echolocation by Insect-Eating Bats , 2001 .

[2]  R. Suthers,et al.  Respiration, Wing-Beat and Ultrasonic Pulse Emission in an Echo-Locating Bat , 1972 .

[3]  M. Holderied,et al.  Bat echolocation calls: adaptation and convergent evolution , 2007, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[4]  H. Schnitzler,et al.  Plasticity in echolocation signals of European pipistrelle bats in search flight: implications for habitat use and prey detection , 1993, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[5]  E. Kalko,et al.  Echolocating Bats Cry Out Loud to Detect Their Prey , 2008, PloS one.

[6]  Charles R. Michael,et al.  The echolocation of flying insects by bats , 1960 .

[7]  Wei Xian,et al.  Adaptive echolocation behavior in bats for the analysis of auditory scenes , 2009, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[8]  E. Kalko,et al.  Insect pursuit, prey capture and echolocation in pipestirelle bats (Microchiroptera) , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[9]  J. Simmons,et al.  Echolocation and pursuit of prey by bats. , 1979, Science.

[10]  C. Moss,et al.  Active Listening for Spatial Orientation in a Complex Auditory Scene , 2006, PLoS biology.

[11]  Hiroshi Riquimaroux,et al.  Echolocation behavior of the Japanese horseshoe bat in pursuit of fluttering prey , 2012, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[12]  D A Waters,et al.  The synchronisation of signal emission with wingbeat during the approach phase in soprano pipistrelles (Pipistrellus pygmaeus). , 2001, The Journal of experimental biology.

[13]  Hiroshi Riquimaroux,et al.  FM echolocating bats shift frequencies to avoid broadcast–echo ambiguity in clutter , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[14]  Jeanette A. Thomas Advances in the Study of Echolocation in Bats and Dolphins , 1999 .

[15]  Cynthia F Moss,et al.  Behavioral responses of big brown bats to dives by praying mantises , 2009, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[16]  C. Moss,et al.  Steering by Hearing: A Bat’s Acoustic Gaze Is Linked to Its Flight Motor Output by a Delayed, Adaptive Linear Law , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Cynthia F. Moss,et al.  Probing the Natural Scene by Echolocation in Bats , 2010, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[18]  PHILIP H.-S. JEN,et al.  Bats avoid moving objects more successfully than stationary ones , 1978, Nature.

[19]  Lasse Jakobsen,et al.  How the bat got its buzz , 2013, Biology Letters.

[20]  C. Moss,et al.  Echolocation behavior of big brown bats, Eptesicus fuscus, in the field and the laboratory. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[21]  M. Fenton,et al.  The influence of roost closure on the roosting and foraging behaviour of Eptesicus fuscus (Chiroptera: Vespertilionidae) , 1986 .

[22]  D. D. Yager,et al.  Free-flight encounters between praying mantids (Parasphendale agrionina) and bats (Eptesicus fuscus) , 2008, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[23]  Lasse Jakobsen,et al.  Echolocating bats emit a highly directional sonar sound beam in the field , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[24]  Salvatore J. Agosta,et al.  Habitat use, diet and roost selection by the Big Brown Bat (Eptesicus fuscus) in North America: a case for conserving an abundant species , 2002 .

[25]  Hans-Ulrich Schnitzler,et al.  Source levels of echolocation signals vary in correlation with wingbeat cycle in landing big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus) , 2010, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[26]  M. E. Anderson,et al.  The energy cost of echolocation in pipistrelle bats (Pipistrettus pipistrellus) , 1989, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[27]  Cynthia F Moss,et al.  Spatial perception and adaptive sonar behavior. , 2010, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[28]  Cynthia F. Moss,et al.  Behavioral Studies of Auditory Information Processing , 1995 .

[29]  Bruce J. Hayward,et al.  Flight Speeds in Western Bats , 1964 .

[30]  J. Simmons,et al.  Interpulse interval modulation by echolocating big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus) in different densities of obstacle clutter , 2009, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[31]  Annemarie Surlykke,et al.  Intensity and directionality of bat echolocation signals , 2013, Front. Physiol..

[32]  E. Kalko,et al.  Dynamic adjustment of biosonar intensity to habitat clutter in the bat Macrophyllum macrophyllum (Phyllostomidae) , 2010, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[33]  C. Moss,et al.  The sonar beam pattern of a flying bat as it tracks tethered insects. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[34]  C F Moss,et al.  Auditory scene analysis by echolocation in bats. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[35]  C. Moss,et al.  Acoustic scanning of natural scenes by echolocation in the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus , 2009, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[36]  D. Griffin Listening in the dark: The acoustic orientation of bats and men. , 1958 .

[37]  H. Schnitzler,et al.  The echolocation and hunting behavior of Daubenton's bat, Myotis daubentoni , 1989, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[38]  H. Schnitzler,et al.  Echolocation signals reflect niche differentiation in five sympatric congeneric bat species , 2004, Nature.

[39]  J. Speakman,et al.  No cost of echolocation for bats in flight , 1991, Nature.

[40]  James A. Simmons,et al.  Versatility of biosonar in the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus , 2001 .