Crowdsourcing Image Schemas

With their potential to map experiental structures from the sensorimotor to the abstract cognitive realm, image schemas are believed to provide an embodied grounding to our cognitive conceptual system, including natural language. Few empirical studies have evaluated humans’ intuitive understanding of image schemas or the coherence of image-schematic annotations of natural language. In this paper we present the results of a human-subjects study in which 100 participants annotate 12 simple English sentences with one or more image schemas. We find that human subjects recruited from a crowdsourcing platform can understand image schema descriptions and use them to perform annotations of texts, but also that in many cases multiple image schema annotations apply to the same simple sentence, a phenomenon we call image schema collocations. This study carries implications both for methodologies of future studies of image schemas, and for the inexpensive and efficient creation of large text corpora with image schema annotations.

[1]  J. Fleiss Measuring nominal scale agreement among many raters. , 1971 .

[2]  M. Banerjee,et al.  Beyond kappa: A review of interrater agreement measures , 1999 .

[3]  R. Wallace The Body in the Mind: The Bodily Basis of Meaning, Imagination, and Reason , 1988 .

[4]  Maria M. Hedblom,et al.  Kinesthetic Mind Reader : A Method to Identify Image Schemas in Natural Language , 2017 .

[5]  Dagmar Gromann,et al.  Body-Mind-Language: Multilingual Knowledge Extraction Based on Embodied Cognition , 2017, AIC.

[6]  H. Kraemer,et al.  Extension of the kappa coefficient. , 1980, Biometrics.

[7]  Beate Hamp,et al.  The Fundamental System of Spatial Schemas in Language , 2022 .

[8]  Daniel Casasanto,et al.  The Meaning of Metaphorical Gestures , 2007 .

[9]  Dagmar Gromann,et al.  In, out and through: formalising some dynamic aspects of the image schema containment , 2018, SAC.

[10]  G. Lakoff,et al.  Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal about the Mind , 1988 .

[11]  Kelly McCormick,et al.  How Linguistic and Cultural Forces Shape Conceptions of Time: English and Mandarin Time in 3D , 2011, Cogn. Sci..

[12]  Roger C. Schank,et al.  Conceptual Information Processing , 2014 .

[13]  Dagmar Gromann,et al.  Image Schemas and Conceptual Dependency Primitives: A Comparison , 2017, JOWO.

[14]  Michael Harrington,et al.  Taking a Stand on the Meanings of Stand: Bodily Experience as Motivation for Polysemy , 1994, J. Semant..

[15]  L. Barsalou Grounded cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[16]  K. Krippendorff Reliability in Content Analysis: Some Common Misconceptions and Recommendations , 2004 .

[17]  Fabian Neuhaus,et al.  Choosing the Right Path: Image Schema Theory as a Foundation for Concept Invention , 2015, J. Artif. Gen. Intell..

[18]  B. Hampe,et al.  Image schemas in Cognitive Linguistics: Introduction , 2005 .

[19]  Maribel Tercedor Sánchez,et al.  The embodied nature of medical concepts: image schemas and language for pain , 2014, Cognitive Processing.

[20]  L. Gleitman,et al.  When English proposes what Greek presupposes: The cross-linguistic encoding of motion events , 2006, Cognition.

[21]  Jean M. Mandler,et al.  On defining image schemas , 2014, Language and Cognition.

[22]  Alan Cienki,et al.  Image schemas and gesture , 2005 .

[23]  J. Mandler How to build a baby: II. Conceptual primitives. , 1992, Psychological review.

[24]  M. Kendall A NEW MEASURE OF RANK CORRELATION , 1938 .