The Neural and Computational Basis of Controlled Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff during Task Performance

People are capable, at will, of trading speed for accuracy when performing a task; they can focus on performing accurately at the cost of being slow, or emphasize speed at the cost of decreased accuracy. Here, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging to investigate the neural correlates of this ability. We show increased baseline activity during speed emphasis in a network of areas related to response preparation and execution, including the premotor areas of the frontal lobe, the basal ganglia, the thalamus, and the dorsolateral prefrontal and left parietal cortices. Furthermore, speed emphasis was associated with reduced transient response-related activation in several of these structures, suggesting that because of the greater baseline activity under speed emphasis, less activation is needed in these structures to reach response threshold, consistent with the assumptions of several computational theories. Moreover, we identify the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex as providing the top-down control signal that increases this baseline activity.

[1]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural Control of Voluntary Movement Initiation , 1996, Science.

[2]  Wayne A. Wickelgren,et al.  Speed-accuracy tradeoff and information processing dynamics , 1977 .

[3]  Pedro F. Santana,et al.  Swarm cognition on off-road autonomous robots , 2011, Swarm Intelligence.

[4]  F. Bloom Principles of Neural Science, 3rd ed , 1993 .

[5]  M. Zorzi,et al.  A computational model of the Simon effect , 1995, Psychological research.

[6]  J R Simon,et al.  Processing symbolic information from a visual display: interference from an irrelevant directional cue. , 1970, Journal of experimental psychology.

[7]  T. Egner,et al.  Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[8]  J. Gold,et al.  Representation of a perceptual decision in developing oculomotor commands , 2000, Nature.

[9]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. , 2006, Psychological review.

[10]  Eliot Hazeltine,et al.  Dissociable Contributions of Prefrontal and Parietal Cortices to Response Selection , 2002, NeuroImage.

[11]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The effect of speed-accuracy strategy on response interference control in Parkinson's disease , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuits: neural substrates of parallel processing , 1990, Trends in Neurosciences.

[13]  G. Kane Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition, vol 1: Foundations, vol 2: Psychological and Biological Models , 1994 .

[14]  E. Donchin,et al.  Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[15]  M. Erb,et al.  Sensorimotor mapping of the human cerebellum: fMRI evidence of somatotopic organization , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[16]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Response of Neurons in the Lateral Intraparietal Area during a Combined Visual Discrimination Reaction Time Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Randy L. Buckner,et al.  Mixed blocked/event-related designs separate transient and sustained activity in fMRI , 2003, NeuroImage.

[18]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.

[19]  D. Friedman,et al.  On why not to rush older adults--relying on reactive cognitive control can effectively reduce errors at the expense of slowed responses. , 2010, Psychophysiology.

[20]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[21]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Involvement of human left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in perceptual decision making is independent of response modality , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[22]  M. Gazzaniga The cognitive neurosciences, 3rd edition , 2004 .

[23]  John R. Anderson,et al.  The role of prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex in task switching. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[24]  Anthony R. Dickinson,et al.  Preparatory Delay Activity in the Monkey Parietal Reach Region Predicts Reach Reaction Times , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  K. H. Britten,et al.  Responses of neurons in macaque MT to stochastic motion signals , 1993, Visual Neuroscience.

[26]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Modeling Response Times for Two-Choice Decisions , 1998 .

[27]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Neural Activity in Macaque Parietal Cortex Reflects Temporal Integration of Visual Motion Signals during Perceptual Decision Making , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  M. Rushworth,et al.  Attention Systems and the Organization of the Human Parietal Cortex , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Naomi Hasegawa,et al.  Thalamocortical and intracortical connections of monkey cingulate motor areas , 2003, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[30]  K M Spencer,et al.  The lateralized readiness potential: relationship between human data and response activation in a connectionist model. , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[31]  Timothy D. Hanks,et al.  Microstimulation of macaque area LIP affects decision-making in a motion discrimination task , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  A. Osman,et al.  On the locus of speed-accuracy trade-off in reaction time: inferences from the lateralized readiness potential. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[33]  Rolf Verleger,et al.  Influence of Time Pressure in a Simple Response Task, a Choice- by-Location Task, and the Simon Task , 2001 .

[34]  Cameron S Carter,et al.  Adding fear to conflict: A general purpose cognitive control network is modulated by trait anxiety , 2010, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[35]  R. Carpenter,et al.  The influence of urgency on decision time , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  Gopal Santhanam,et al.  Preparatory activity in premotor and motor cortex reflects the speed of the upcoming reach. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  Robert G Pachella,et al.  The Interpretation of Reaction Time in Information-Processing Research 1 , 1973, Human Information Processing.

[38]  B. Feige,et al.  The Role of Higher-Order Motor Areas in Voluntary Movement as Revealed by High-Resolution EEG and fMRI , 1999, NeuroImage.

[39]  A. Osman,et al.  Mechanisms of speed–accuracy tradeoff: evidence from covert motor processes , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[40]  M. Alexander,et al.  Principles of Neural Science , 1981 .

[41]  Vincent van Veen,et al.  A NEUROIMAGING APPROACH TO THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN ATTENTION AND SPEED-ACCURACY TRADEOFF , 2006 .

[42]  Philip Holmes,et al.  Rapid decision threshold modulation by reward rate in a neural network , 2006, Neural Networks.

[43]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[44]  Cameron S Carter,et al.  Cognitive control involved in overcoming prepotent response tendencies and switching between tasks. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[45]  W. Newsome,et al.  Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  J. Schall On building a bridge between brain and behavior. , 2004, Annual review of psychology.

[47]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.

[48]  James L. McClelland,et al.  On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. , 1990, Psychological review.

[49]  P. Strick,et al.  Imaging the premotor areas , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[50]  L. M. Warner,et al.  The Neural Mechanisms for Minimizing Cross-Modal Distraction , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  J. Gold,et al.  Banburismus and the Brain Decoding the Relationship between Sensory Stimuli, Decisions, and Reward , 2002, Neuron.

[52]  S. Bouisset,et al.  [Voluntary movement]. , 1953, Journal de physiologie.

[53]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[54]  D Le Bihan,et al.  The Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex (dlpfc) Plays a Key Role in Working Memory (wm). yet Its Precise Contribution , 2022 .

[55]  B A J Reddi,et al.  Accuracy, information, and response time in a saccadic decision task. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[56]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: foundations , 1986 .

[57]  G. Glover Deconvolution of Impulse Response in Event-Related BOLD fMRI1 , 1999, NeuroImage.

[58]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  Searching for “the Top” in Top-Down Control , 2005, Neuron.

[59]  David E. Irwin,et al.  The dynamics of cognition and action: mental processes inferred from speed-accuracy decomposition. , 1988, Psychological review.

[60]  Kristina M. Visscher,et al.  A Core System for the Implementation of Task Sets , 2006, Neuron.

[61]  A M Graybiel,et al.  The basal ganglia and adaptive motor control. , 1994, Science.

[62]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Psychophysiological and Modulatory Interactions in Neuroimaging , 1997, NeuroImage.

[63]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Prefrontal and midline interactions mediating behavioural control , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[64]  J. Schall Neural correlates of decision processes: neural and mental chronometry , 2003, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[65]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A parallel distributed processing approach to automaticity. , 1992, The American journal of psychology.

[66]  J. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. , 2000, Science.

[67]  R. Bogacz,et al.  The neural basis of the speed–accuracy tradeoff , 2010, Trends in Neurosciences.

[68]  P. Strick,et al.  Motor areas of the medial wall: a review of their location and functional activation. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[69]  M. Gazzaniga,et al.  The new cognitive neurosciences , 2000 .

[70]  R. Passingham,et al.  The Attentional Role of the Left Parietal Cortex: The Distinct Lateralization and Localization of Motor Attention in the Human Brain , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[71]  Xiao-Jing Wang,et al.  Cortico–basal ganglia circuit mechanism for a decision threshold in reaction time tasks , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[72]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.