Auditory Statistical Learning During Concurrent Physical Exercise and the Tolerance for Pitch, Tempo, and Rhythm Changes.

Previous studies suggest that statistical learning is preserved when acoustic changes are made to auditory sequences. However, statistical learning effects can vary with and without concurrent exercise. The present study examined how concurrent physical exercise influences auditory statistical learning when acoustical and temporal changes are made to auditory sequences. Participants were presented with the 500-tone sequences based on a Markov chain while cycling or resting in ignored and attended conditions. Learning effects were evaluated using a familiarity test with four types of short tone series: tone series in which stimuli were same as 500-tone sequence and three tone series in which frequencies, tempo, or rhythm was changed. We suggested that, regardless of attention, concurrent exercise interferes with tolerance in statistical learning for rhythm, rather than tempo changes. There may be specific relationships among statistical learning, rhythm perception, and motor system underlying physical exercise.

[1]  M. Goldsmith,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996 .

[2]  Gábor P. Háden,et al.  Newborn infants detect the beat in music , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[3]  M. Yumoto,et al.  Statistical learning of an auditory sequence and reorganization of acquired knowledge: A time course of word segmentation and ordering , 2017, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  Tatsuya Daikoku,et al.  Physical fitness modulates incidental but not intentional statistical learning of simultaneous auditory sequences during concurrent physical exercise , 2017, Neurological research.

[5]  David I. Anderson,et al.  Concurrent Movement Impairs Incidental But Not Intentional Statistical Learning , 2015, Cogn. Sci..

[6]  E. Rosa,et al.  EXPLICITNESS, INTAKE, AND THE ISSUE OF AWARENESS , 1999, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.

[7]  Stefan Koelsch,et al.  Under the hood of statistical learning: A statistical MMN reflects the magnitude of transitional probabilities in auditory sequences , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[8]  B. Ross,et al.  Beta-Band Oscillations Represent Auditory Beat and Its Metrical Hierarchy in Perception and Imagery , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  D. Schön,et al.  Music training for the development of speech segmentation. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[10]  N. Uth,et al.  Estimation of V̇O2max from the ratio between HRmax and HRrest – the Heart Rate Ratio Method , 2004, European Journal of Applied Physiology.

[11]  Katie Overy,et al.  Dyslexia and music: measuring musical timing skills. , 2003, Dyslexia.

[12]  Rod Ellis,et al.  The Structural Syllabus and Second Language Acquisition , 1993 .

[13]  A. Peres,et al.  Physical fitness modulates the expression of CD39 and CD73 on CD4+ CD25− and CD4+ CD25+ T cells following high intensity interval exercise , 2019, Journal of cellular biochemistry.

[14]  Masato Yumoto,et al.  Single, but not dual, attention facilitates statistical learning of two concurrent auditory sequences , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[15]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  M. Yumoto,et al.  Implicit and explicit statistical learning of tone sequences across spectral shifts , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  Fleur L. Bouwer,et al.  Disentangling beat perception from sequential learning and examining the influence of attention and musical abilities on ERP responses to rhythm , 2016, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  M. Yumoto,et al.  Pitch-class distribution modulates the statistical learning of atonal chord sequences , 2016, Brain and Cognition.

[19]  Masato Yumoto,et al.  Statistical learning of music- and language-like sequences and tolerance for spectral shifts , 2015, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[20]  R. Dekeyser Implicit and Explicit Learning , 2008 .

[21]  P. Wolff Timing precision and rhythm in developmental dyslexia , 2002 .

[22]  Fleur L. Bouwer,et al.  Beat Processing Is Pre-Attentive for Metrically Simple Rhythms with Clear Accents: An ERP Study , 2014, PloS one.

[23]  P. Perruchet,et al.  Implicit learning and statistical learning: one phenomenon, two approaches , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  R. Leow Toward operationalizing the process of attention in SLA: Evidence for Tomlin and Villa's (1994) finegrained analysis of attention , 1998, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[25]  B. Ross,et al.  Internalized Timing of Isochronous Sounds Is Represented in Neuromagnetic Beta Oscillations , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  J. Etnier,et al.  The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: A meta-analysis , 2012, Brain Research.

[27]  Angela D. Friederici,et al.  Hierarchical artificial grammar processing engages Broca's area , 2008, NeuroImage.

[28]  Gábor P. Háden,et al.  Probing Attentive and Preattentive Emergent Meter in Adult Listeners without Extensive Music Training , 2009 .

[29]  A. Roepstorff,et al.  Predictive coding of music – Brain responses to rhythmic incongruity , 2009, Cortex.

[30]  D. Berry,et al.  Implicit and explicit processes in a second-language learning task , 1994 .

[31]  Robert A. Robergs,et al.  The surprising history of the "HRmax=220-age" equation , 2002 .

[32]  P. Robinson Learning Simple and Complex Second Language Rules Under Implicit, Incidental, Rule-Search, and Instructed Conditions , 1996, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.

[33]  Dennis H. Klatt,et al.  Software for a cascade/parallel formant synthesizer , 1980 .

[34]  Tatsuya Daikoku,et al.  The variation of hemodynamics relative to listening to consonance or dissonance during chord progression , 2012, Neurological research.