Kinetic contours in infants' visual perception.

3-month-old infants' perception of "camouflaged" forms that are only visible when moving was studied. Displays were used in which figure and ground had an identical random dot texture, and no edge indicated the form of the figure. The form was invisible when stationary. Discrimination of 2 different forms was tested (a) when the forms were visible only through motion, and (b) when the forms appeared as stationary white figures on black ground. The babies discriminated the forms in both conditions. Furthermore, when infants were habituated to one of the moving forms and subsequently presented with the same and a new static form, they looked longer at the new form. This indicates that they recognized the static form as either the same or different from the moving form seen before, although the optical sources of information were completely different. At 3 months, infants can therefore effectively use kinetic information to organize the visual input in higher-order structures.

[1]  J. Gibson The Senses Considered As Perceptual Systems , 1967 .

[2]  George A. Kaplan,et al.  Kinetic disruption of optical texture: The perception of depth at an edge , 1969 .

[3]  B. Julesz Foundations of Cyclopean Perception , 1971 .

[4]  T G Bower,et al.  Development of the object concept as manifested in changes in the tracking behavior of infants between 7 and 20 weeks of age. , 1971, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[5]  G. Johansson Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .

[6]  Rudolf Arnheim,et al.  Gesetze des Sehens , 1977 .

[7]  A Yonas,et al.  Development of sensitivity to pictorial depth. , 1978, Science.

[8]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[9]  B Rogers,et al.  Motion Parallax as an Independent Cue for Depth Perception , 1979, Perception.

[10]  A. Yonas,et al.  Sensitivity of 5- and 7-month-old infants to pictorial depth information , 1981 .

[11]  A. Yonas 11 – Infants' Responses to Optical Information for Collision , 1981 .

[12]  R. Fox,et al.  The perception of biological motion by human infants. , 1982, Science.

[13]  C. Baker,et al.  Does segregation of differently moving areas depend on relative or absolute displacement? , 1982, Vision Research.

[14]  Albert Yonas,et al.  Infants' sensitivity to familiar size as information for distance. , 1982 .

[15]  E. Spelke,et al.  Perception of partly occluded objects in infancy , 1983, Cognitive Psychology.

[16]  A. Yonas,et al.  Infants' sensitivity to accretion and deletion of texture as information for depth at an edge. , 1984, Child development.

[17]  B. Bertenthal,et al.  Infant sensitivity to figural coherence in biomechanical motions. , 1984, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[18]  E. Spelke,et al.  Gestalt relations and object perception in infancy , 1984 .

[19]  Infants' recognition of a face revealed through motion: Contribution of internal facial movements and head movement , 1984 .

[20]  A. Yonas,et al.  Infants' perception of pictorially specified interposition. , 1984, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[21]  J. M. Byrne,et al.  The perception of stimulus shape: the influence of velocity of stimulus movement. , 1984, Child development.

[22]  R. Kaufmann-Hayoz,et al.  Development of infants' sensitivity for slow and rapid motions , 1985 .

[23]  R. Kaufmann-Hayoz,et al.  Infants' recognition of a face revealed through motion: contribution of internal facial movement and head movement. , 1987, Journal of experimental child psychology.