Kinematic properties of rapid hand movements in a knob turning task

Abstract. In order to understand how the central nervous system controls the kinematics of rapid finger and hand movements, we studied the motions of subjects turning a knob to light-emitting diode targets, similar to tuning a radio dial. On many trials, subjects turned the knob with a single, smooth, and regular motion as revealed by the angular position and velocity trajectories, but on others, subjects produced irregularities in the kinematics. Like many past studies, we interpreted these irregularities as discrete corrective submovements. Unlike other studies, we used a direct, objective algorithm to identify overlapping submovements, detecting appreciable inflections in the acceleration traces by examining zero crossings in their derivatives, jerk and snap. The movements without overlapping submovements on average had a near symmetric, bell-shaped velocity profile that was independent of speed, and which matched the theoretical minimum jerk velocity very closely. We proposed three plausible mechanisms for altering the shape of movement kinematics, and implemented a mass-spring model with non-linear damping to explore the possibilities. Although there was relatively little variability in the shape and symmetry of movements across trials, there was a fair amount of variability in their amplitude. We show that subjects attempted to eliminate the need for corrective submovements by making more accurate primary movements with practice, but that the variability inherent in rapid movements dictated the need for corrective submovements. Subjects used corrective submovements to improve final endpoint accuracy while reducing endpoint variability, resulting in higher task success rates.

[1]  Robert Sessions Woodworth,et al.  THE ACCURACY OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENT , 1899 .

[2]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[3]  Steven W. Keele,et al.  Movement control in skilled motor performance. , 1968 .

[4]  H. Zelaznik,et al.  Motor-output variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. , 1979, Psychological review.

[5]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Human arm trajectory formation. , 1982, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  E. R. Crossman,et al.  Feedback Control of Hand-Movement and Fitts' Law , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[7]  Y. Lamarre,et al.  Fast ballistic arm movements triggered by visual, auditory, and somesthetic stimuli in the monkey. I. Activity of precentral cortical neurons. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Posture control and trajectory formation during arm movement , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[9]  T. Flash,et al.  The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[10]  J. Houk,et al.  Relation between red nucleus discharge and movement parameters in trained macaque monkeys. , 1985, The Journal of physiology.

[11]  C. Atkeson,et al.  Kinematic features of unrestrained vertical arm movements , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[12]  A. G. Feldman Once More on the Equilibrium-Point Hypothesis (λ Model) for Motor Control , 1986 .

[13]  A. G. Feldman Once more on the equilibrium-point hypothesis (lambda model) for motor control. , 1986, Journal of motor behavior.

[14]  R. Marteniuk,et al.  Kinematic and electromyographic changes that occur as a function of learning a time-constrained aiming task. , 1986, Journal of motor behavior.

[15]  L. Stark,et al.  Simulation studies of descending and reflex control of fast movements. , 1987, Journal of motor behavior.

[16]  R A Abrams,et al.  Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. , 1988, Psychological review.

[17]  G. Gottlieb,et al.  Strategies for the control of voluntary movements with one mechanical degree of freedom , 1989, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[18]  J. Cooke,et al.  Movement-related phasic muscle activation. I. Relations with temporal profile of movement. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  J. Houk,et al.  Nonlinear damping of limb motion , 1990 .

[20]  N. Hogan Mechanical Impedance of Single- and Multi-Articular Systems , 1990 .

[21]  T. Flash,et al.  Arm Trajectory Modifications During Reaching Towards Visual Targets , 1991, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  R. Stein,et al.  Factors that determine the magnitude and time course of human H- reflexes in locomotion , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[23]  T. Milner,et al.  A model for the generation of movements requiring endpoint precision , 1992, Neuroscience.

[24]  P. Brodal The Central Nervous System , 1992 .

[25]  A. Barto,et al.  Distributed motor commands in the limb premotor network , 1993, Trends in Neurosciences.

[26]  Satinder Singh,et al.  Distributed Representation of Limb Motor Programs in Arrays of Adjustable Pattern Generators , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  M. Kawato Optimization and learning in neural networks for formation and control of coordinated movement , 1993 .

[28]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[29]  J. Pratt,et al.  Rapid aimed limb movements: age differences and practice effects in component submovements. , 1994, Psychology and aging.

[30]  James C. Houk,et al.  Controlling a Nonlinear Spring-Mass System with a Cerebellar Model , 1994 .

[31]  S P Wise,et al.  Distributed modular architectures linking basal ganglia, cerebellum, and cerebral cortex: their role in planning and controlling action. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.

[32]  J. Houk,et al.  Motor co‐ordinates in primate red nucleus: preferential relation to muscle activation versus kinematic variables. , 1995, The Journal of physiology.

[33]  N. Berthier Learning to reach: A mathematical model. , 1996 .

[34]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Manual interception of moving targets II. On-line control of overlapping submovements , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  N. Berthier Analysis of Reaching for Stationary and Moving Objects in the Human Infant , 1997 .

[36]  Gideon F. Inbar,et al.  A model for learning human reaching movements , 1996, Biological Cybernetics.

[37]  R Plamondon,et al.  Speed/accuracy trade-offs in target-directed movements , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[38]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Making smooth moves , 2022 .

[39]  N. Hogan,et al.  Robot-aided neurorehabilitation. , 1998, IEEE transactions on rehabilitation engineering : a publication of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society.

[40]  Etienne Burdet,et al.  Quantization of human motions and learning of accurate movements , 1998, Biological Cybernetics.

[41]  James C. Houk,et al.  A Cerebellar Model of Timing and Prediction in the Control of Reaching , 1999, Neural Computation.

[42]  G. Inbar,et al.  The Use of a Nonlinear Muscle Model in Explaining the Relationship Between Duration, Amplitude, and Peak Velocity of Human Rapid Movements. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.