Varieties of grapheme-colour synaesthesia: A new theory of phenomenological and behavioural differences

Recent research has suggested that not all grapheme-colour synaesthetes are alike. One suggestion is that they can be divided, phenomenologically, in terms of whether the colours are experienced in external or internal space (projector-associator distinction). Another suggestion is that they can be divided according to whether it is the perceptual or conceptual attributes of a stimulus that is critical (higher-lower distinction). This study compares the behavioural performance of 7 projector and 7 associator synaesthetes. We demonstrate that this distinction does not map on to behavioural traits expected from the higher-lower distinction. We replicate previous research showing that projectors are faster at naming their synaesthetic colours than veridical colours, and that associators show the reverse profile. Synaesthetes who project colours into external space but not on to the surface of the grapheme behave like associators on this task. In a second task, graphemes presented briefly in the periphery are more likely to elicit reports of colour in projectors than associators, but the colours only tend to be accurate when the grapheme itself is also accurately identified. We propose an alternative model of individual differences in grapheme-colour synaesthesia that emphasises the role of different spatial reference frames in synaesthetic perception. In doing so, we attempt to bring the synaesthesia literature closer to current models of non-synaesthetic perception, attention and binding.

[1]  J. Marshall,et al.  Left neglect for near but not far space in man , 1991, Nature.

[2]  Jonathan Winawer,et al.  Synesthetic Colors Determined by Having Colored Refrigerator Magnets in Childhood , 2006, Cortex.

[3]  17 Two Neural Correlates of Consciousness , 2007 .

[4]  C. Kennard,et al.  The anatomy of visual neglect , 2003 .

[5]  N. Sagiv,et al.  What is the relationship between synaesthesia and visuo-spatial number forms? , 2006, Cognition.

[6]  M. Tanaka,et al.  Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[7]  R. Cohen Kadosh,et al.  When a line is a number: Color yields magnitude information in a digit-color synesthete , 2006, Neuroscience.

[8]  Daniel Smilek,et al.  The Importance of Individual Differences in Grapheme-Color Synesthesia , 2005, Neuron.

[9]  Jamie Ward,et al.  Synaesthesia: The Prevalence of Atypical Cross-Modal Experiences , 2006, Perception.

[10]  B. Laeng,et al.  Does Color Synesthesia Pose a Paradox for Early-Selection Theories of Attention? , 2004, Psychological science.

[11]  Geoffrey M. Boynton,et al.  Individual Differences among Grapheme-Color Synesthetes: Brain-Behavior Correlations , 2005, Neuron.

[12]  Avishai Henik,et al.  When Blue is Larger than Red: Colors Influence Numerical Cognition in Synesthesia , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Contrast Affects the Strength of Synesthetic Colors , 2006, Cortex.

[14]  D. Levi,et al.  The effect of similarity and duration on spatial interaction in peripheral vision. , 1994, Spatial vision.

[15]  C. Guariglia,et al.  Personal and extrapersonal space: A case of neglect dissociation , 1992, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  C. Brinkman,et al.  Do Synaesthetic Colours Act as Unique Features in Visual Search? , 2006, Cortex.

[17]  Jason B. Mattingley,et al.  Unconscious priming eliminates automatic binding of colour and alphanumeric form in synaesthesia , 2001, Nature.

[18]  Christopher T. Lovelace,et al.  Mechanisms of synesthesia: cognitive and physiological constraints , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[19]  FRANCIS GALTON,et al.  Visualised Numerals , 1880, Nature.

[20]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[21]  D. Smilek,et al.  Synaesthetic photisms guide attention , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[22]  Jamie Ward,et al.  A comparison of lexical-gustatory and grapheme-colour synaesthesia , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[23]  C. Mills,et al.  DIGIT SYNAESTHESIA: A CASE STUDY USING A STROOP-TYPE TEST , 1999 .

[24]  Richard B. Ivry,et al.  Coming Unbound: Disrupting Automatic Integration of Synesthetic Color and Graphemes by Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation of the Right Parietal Lobe , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  D. Smilek,et al.  Towards a Synergistic Understanding of Synaesthesia Combining Current Experimental Findings With Synaesthetes' Subjective Descriptions , 2002 .

[26]  J. Simner Review: Synesthesia: Perspectives from Cognitive Neuroscience , 2005 .

[27]  D. Smilek,et al.  Colored Photisms Prevent Object-Substitution Masking in Digit-Color Synesthesia , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[28]  Robert H. Logie,et al.  Visual perception and verbal descriptions as sources for generating mental representations: Evidence from representational neglect , 2002, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[29]  Vincent Walsh,et al.  Disruption of synaesthesia following TMS of the right posterior parietal cortex , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  Daniel Smilek,et al.  Five plus two equals yellow , 2000, Nature.

[31]  Paolo Bartolomeo,et al.  The Relationship Between Visual Perception and Visual Mental Imagery: A Reappraisal of the Neuropsychological Evidence , 2002, Cortex.

[32]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Attention to within-object and between-object spatial representations: multiple sites for visual selection , 1994 .

[33]  Gianna Cocchini,et al.  The Fluff Test: A simple task to assess body representation neglect , 2001 .

[34]  N. Sagiv,et al.  Synaesthesia for Finger Counting and Dice Patterns: A Case of Higher Synaesthesia? , 2007, Neurocase.

[35]  T Landis,et al.  Near and far visual space in unilateral neglect , 1998, Annals of neurology.

[36]  A. Treisman Features and Objects: The Fourteenth Bartlett Memorial Lecture , 1988, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[37]  J. Downey A Case of Colored Gustation , 1911 .

[38]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Psychophysical investigations into the neural basis of synaesthesia , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[39]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[40]  Jamie Ward,et al.  Lexical-gustatory synaesthesia: linguistic and conceptual factors , 2003, Cognition.

[41]  Daniel Smilek,et al.  Not all synaesthetes are created equal: Projector versus associator synaesthetes , 2004, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience.

[42]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Synaesthesia? A window into perception, thought and language , 2001 .

[43]  Randolph Blake,et al.  The perceptual reality of synesthetic colors , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[44]  Daniel Smilek,et al.  Synaesthetic Photisms Influence Visual Perception , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  Vincent Walsh A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[46]  Ashok S. Jansari,et al.  Number Synaesthesia: When Hearing “Four Plus Five” Looks Like Gold , 2006, Cortex.

[47]  Jamie Ward,et al.  Non-random associations of graphemes to colours in synaesthetic and non-synaesthetic populations , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[48]  Jeffrey Heer,et al.  Does Binding of Synesthetic Color to the Evoking Grapheme Require Attention? , 2006, Cortex.