Role of the different frontal lobe areas in the control of the horizontal component of memory-guided saccades in man

Two paradigms of memory-guided saccades were studied in 14 patients with focal vascular lesions affecting either the frontal eye field (FEF), or the supplementary eye field (SEF) or Brodmann's area 46 in the prefrontal cortex (PFC), and in 13 age-matched control subjects. In the first paradigm, the subject had to remember the position of a visual target with the body immobile and, in the second, the position towards which gaze was directed before a body rotation, i.e. with a vestibular input. In control subjects, the percentage of error in saccade accuracy (horizontal component) was greater in the second than in the first paradigm (37% and 14% on average, respectively). Compared with controls, amplitude error was significantly increased in the FEF group for the first paradigm only, in the SEF group for the second paradigm only, and in the PFC group for both paradigms. These results are consistent with (1) the PFC providing an improvement in the utilization by the saccade system of the visual and vestibular signals used in the two paradigms, and (2) the FEF and SEF providing an improvement in the utilization of visual signals in the first paradigm and vestibular signals in the second paradigm, respectively. Furthermore, from these findings and experimental data, it may be hypothesized (1) that the PFC is a part of the network contributing to short-term memorization of both visual and vestibular signals, and (2) that the FEF and SEF control two different types of memory-guided saccades, with separate calculation modes to determine their amplitude.

[1]  B. Larsen,et al.  Cortical activation pattern during saccadic eye movements in humans: Localization by focal cerebral blood flow increases , 1979, Annals of neurology.

[2]  J. L. Conway,et al.  Deficits in eye movements following frontal eye-field and superior colliculus ablations. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  M. Raichle,et al.  The role of cerebral cortex in the generation of voluntary saccades: a positron emission tomographic study. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  J. van der Steen,et al.  Effects of unilateral frontal eye-field lesions on eye-head coordination in monkey. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  Evidence for a supplementary eye field. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Common cortical and subcortical targets of the dorsolateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices in the rhesus monkey: evidence for a distributed neural network subserving spatially guided behavior , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[7]  M E Goldberg,et al.  Frontal eye field efferents in the macaque monkey: I. Subcortical pathways and topography of striatal and thalamic terminal fields , 1988, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[8]  C. Bruce,et al.  Frontal eye field efferents in the macaque monkey: II. Topography of terminal fields in midbrain and pons , 1988, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[9]  R. Andersen,et al.  Posterior parietal cortex. , 1989, Reviews of oculomotor research.

[10]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Posterior parietal cortex in rhesus monkey: II. Evidence for segregated corticocortical networks linking sensory and limbic areas with the frontal lobe , 1989, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[11]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Posterior parietal cortex in rhesus monkey: I. Parcellation of areas based on distinctive limbic and sensory corticocortical connections , 1989, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[12]  Andrew R. Mitz,et al.  Eye-movement representation in the frontal lobe of rhesus monkeys , 1989, Neuroscience Letters.

[13]  D. Sparks The neural encoding of the location of targets for saccadic eye movements. , 1989, The Journal of experimental biology.

[14]  C. Bruce,et al.  Primate frontal eye fields. III. Maintenance of a spatially accurate saccade signal. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Visuospatial coding in primate prefrontal neurons revealed by oculomotor paradigms. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  L. Fogassi,et al.  Eye position effects on visual, memory, and saccade-related activity in areas LIP and 7a of macaque , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[17]  J Schlag,et al.  Primate supplementary eye field: I. Comparative aspects of mesencephalic and pontine connections , 1990, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[18]  S. Rivaud,et al.  Impairment of sequences of memory‐guided saccades after supplementary motor area lesions , 1990, Annals of neurology.

[19]  J. Schall,et al.  Neuronal activity related to visually guided saccadic eye movements in the supplementary motor area of rhesus monkeys. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  J. Schall Neuronal activity related to visually guided saccades in the frontal eye fields of rhesus monkeys: comparison with supplementary eye fields. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  Y Agid,et al.  Cortical control of reflexive visually-guided saccades. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[22]  G. S. Russo,et al.  Effect of eye position within the orbit on electrically elicited saccadic eye movements: a comparison of the macaque monkey's frontal and supplementary eye fields. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  D. Sparks,et al.  Saccades to remembered targets exhibit enhanced orbital position effects in monkeys , 2001, Vision Research.

[24]  P. H. Schiller,et al.  Conditional task-related responses in monkey dorsomedial frontal cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  J. Schlag,et al.  Supplementary eye field: influence of eye position on neural signals of fixation , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  B. Segal,et al.  Adaptive modification of vestibularly perceived rotation , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  B. Gaymard,et al.  Cortical control of memory-guided saccades in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.