Location matters: Why target location impacts performance in orientation tasks

This research explores human performance in a spatial orientation task. In three experiments, participants saw a target highlighted in a visual scene and were asked to locate it on a map of the space. Across all of the experiments, the target’s location in the visual scene influenced the participants’ response times. Generally, response times increased when the target was located farther away from the viewer, when the target was farther to one side or the other, and when more distractors were nearby. However, there were important exceptions to these findings, suggesting that participants encode the location of a target hierarchically, using different features of the space depending on the target’s particular location. We conclude that participants perform such tasks by extracting a description from the egocentric view and then transforming that description to allow them to find the target on the map.

[1]  U. Neisser Decision-time without reaction-time: Experiments in visual scanning. , 1963 .

[2]  R. Shepard,et al.  Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects , 1971, Science.

[3]  A. Siegel,et al.  The development of spatial representations of large-scale environments. , 1975, Advances in child development and behavior.

[4]  E. A. Maxwell Geometry by transformations , 1975 .

[5]  S. Vandenberg,et al.  Mental Rotations, a Group Test of Three-Dimensional Spatial Visualization , 1978, Perceptual and motor skills.

[6]  Albert C. Stevens,et al.  Distortions in judged spatial relations , 1978, Cognitive Psychology.

[7]  E. Sadalla,et al.  Reference points in spatial cognition. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[8]  H E Egeth,et al.  Right-left confusion in the adult: A verbal labeling effect , 1981, Memory & cognition.

[9]  D. L. Hintzman,et al.  Orientation in cognitive maps , 1981, Cognitive Psychology.

[10]  Irwin N. Jankovic,et al.  Principles of spatial problem solving. , 1982 .

[11]  Clark C. Presson Strategies in Spatial Reasoning. , 1982 .

[12]  Gary L. Allen,et al.  Expression of Configurational Knowledge of Large-Scale Environments , 1984 .

[13]  R. Shepard,et al.  Upward direction, mental rotation, and discrimination of left and right turns in maps , 1984, Cognition.

[14]  J. Jonides,et al.  Evidence of hierarchies in cognitive maps , 1985, Memory & cognition.

[15]  T. McNamara Mental representations of spatial relations , 1986, Cognitive Psychology.

[16]  D. R. Montello,et al.  Landmarks and the coordination and integration of spatial information , 1988 .

[17]  Clark C. Presson,et al.  Points of reference in spatial cognition: Stalking the elusive landmark* , 1988 .

[18]  P. Jolicoeur Mental rotation and the identification of disoriented objects. , 1988, Canadian journal of psychology.

[19]  C. E. Bethell-Fox,et al.  Mental rotation: effects of stimulus complexity and familiarity , 1988 .

[20]  J. Rieser Access to knowledge of spatial structure at novel points of observation. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[21]  Pierre Jolicoeur,et al.  "Mental rotation and the identification of disoriented objects": Erratum. , 1989 .

[22]  Barbara Tversky,et al.  Searching Imagined Environments , 1990 .

[23]  L. Boer Mental rotation in perspective problems. , 1991, Acta psychologica.

[24]  R. D. Easton,et al.  Object-array structure, frames of reference, and retrieval of spatial knowledge. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[25]  Clark C. Presson,et al.  Orientation Specificity in Representations of Place , 1997 .

[26]  T. McNamara,et al.  Symmetry and Asymmetry of Human Spatial Memory , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[27]  C. Freksa,et al.  Spatial Cognition, An Interdisciplinary Approach to Representing and Processing Spatial Knowledge , 1998 .

[28]  Roberta L. Klatzky,et al.  Allocentric and Egocentric Spatial Representations: Definitions, Distinctions, and Interconnections , 1998, Spatial Cognition.

[29]  Richard B. Chase,et al.  THE ACQUISITION OF ROUTE AND SURVEY KNOWLEDGE FROM COMPUTER MODELS , 1999 .

[30]  Anthony E. Richardson,et al.  Spatial knowledge acquisition from maps and from navigation in real and virtual environments , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[31]  Leo Gugerty,et al.  Keeping North in Mind: How Navigators Reason about Cardinal Directions , 2000 .

[32]  D R Proffitt,et al.  Updating displays after imagined object and viewer rotations. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[33]  Timothy P. McNamara,et al.  Systems of Spatial Reference in Human Memory , 2001, Cognitive Psychology.

[34]  M. Chun,et al.  Perceptual constraints on implicit learning of spatial context , 2002 .

[35]  Marvin M Chun,et al.  Implicit scene learning is viewpoint dependent , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[36]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Orientation Tasks with Multiple Views of Space: Strategies and Performance , 2004, Spatial Cogn. Comput..

[37]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Strategic Differences in the Coordination of Different Views of Space , 2019, Proceedings of the Twenty-Fourth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.