Is a picture worth a thousand words? Preference for auditory modality in young children.

Linguistic labels play an important role in young children's conceptual organization: When 2 entities share a label, people expect these entities to share many other properties. Two classes of explanations of the importance of labels seem plausible: a language-specific and a general auditory explanation. The general auditory explanation argues that the importance of labels stems from a privileged processing status of auditory input (as compared with visual input) for young children. This hypothesis was tested and supported in 4 experiments. When auditory and visual stimuli were presented separately, 4-year-olds were likely to process both kinds of stimuli, whereas when auditory and visual stimuli were presented simultaneously, 4-year-olds were more likely to process auditory stimuli than visual stimuli.

[1]  G. Gottlieb Development of species identification in birds : an inquiry into the prenatal determinants of perception , 1971 .

[2]  J. Birnholz,et al.  The development of human fetal hearing. , 1983, Science.

[3]  R. Nosofsky Attention, similarity, and the identification-categorization relationship. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[4]  E. Markman,et al.  Categories and induction in young children , 1986, Cognition.

[5]  David J. Lewkowicz,et al.  Sensory dominance in infants. II: Ten-month-old infants' response to auditory-visual compounds , 1988 .

[6]  R N Aslin,et al.  Perceptual development. , 1988, Annual review of psychology.

[7]  Ellen M. Markman,et al.  Categorization and Naming in Children: Problems of Induction , 1989 .

[8]  John D. Coley,et al.  Perspectives on language and thought: Language and categorization: The acquisition of natural kind terms , 1991 .

[9]  Kenneth Roberts,et al.  Linguistic versus attentional influences on nonlinguistic categorization in 15-month-old infants , 1991 .

[10]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Object properties and knowledge in early lexical learning. , 1991, Child development.

[11]  David J. Lewkowicz,et al.  Development of intersensory perception in human infants. , 1994 .

[12]  S. Waxman,et al.  Words as Invitations to Form Categories: Evidence from 12- to 13-Month-Old Infants , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.

[13]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Naming in young children: a dumb attentional mechanism? , 1996, Cognition.

[14]  S. Waxman,et al.  Do words facilitate object categorization in 9-month-old infants? , 1997, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[15]  P. Jusczyk The discovery of spoken language , 1997 .

[16]  M. Casasola,et al.  Acquisition of word-object associations by 14-month-old infants. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[17]  M. Casasola,et al.  Acquisition of word-object associations by 14-month-old infants. , 1998 .

[18]  A. Woodward,et al.  Infants' learning about words and sounds in relation to objects. , 1999, Child development.

[19]  V. Sloutsky,et al.  How much does a shared name make things similar? Part 1. Linguistic labels and the development of similarity judgment. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[20]  M. Ma,et al.  Responses to innocuous, but not noxious, somatosensory stimulation by neurons in the ferret superior colliculus. , 2000 .

[21]  Responses to innocuous, but not noxious, somatosensory stimulation by neurons in the ferret superior colliculus. , 2000, Somatosensory & motor research.

[22]  Effects of Linguistic and Perceptual Information on Categorization in Young Children - eScholarship , 2001 .

[23]  V. Sloutsky,et al.  How much does a shared name make things similar? Linguistic labels, similarity, and the development of inductive inference. , 2001, Child development.

[24]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  How children know the relevant properties for generalizing object names , 2002 .