Development of abstract mathematical reasoning: the case of algebra

Algebra typically represents the students’ first encounter with abstract mathematical reasoning and it therefore causes significant difficulties for students who still reason concretely. The aim of the present study was to investigate the developmental trajectory of the students’ ability to solve simple algebraic equations. 311 participants between the ages of 13 and 17 were given a computerized test of equation rearrangement. Equations consisted of an unknown and two other elements (numbers or letters), and the operations of multiplication/division. The obtained results showed that younger participants are less accurate and slower in solving equations with letters (symbols) than those with numbers. This difference disappeared for older participants (16–17 years), suggesting that they had reached an abstract reasoning level, at least for this simple task. A corresponding conclusion arises from the analysis of their strategies which suggests that younger participants mostly used concrete strategies such as inserting numbers, while older participants typically used more abstract, rule-based strategies. These results indicate that the development of algebraic thinking is a process which unfolds over a long period of time. In agreement with previous research, we can conclude that, on average, children at the age of 15–16 transition from using concrete to abstract strategies while solving the algebra problems addressed within the present study. A better understanding of the timing and speed of students’ transition from concrete arithmetic reasoning to abstract algebraic reasoning might help in designing better curricula and teaching materials that would ease that transition.

[1]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[2]  J. Rapoport,et al.  Structural MRI of Pediatric Brain Development: What Have We Learned and Where Are We Going? , 2010, Neuron.

[3]  Anton E. Lawson,et al.  A Review of Research on Formal Reasoning and Science Teaching. , 1985 .

[4]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Gender Differences in Predictors of College Mathematics Performance and in College Mathematics Course Grades. , 1991 .

[5]  Andreja Bubić,et al.  EYE MOVEMENTS REVEAL STUDENTS’ STRATEGIES IN SIMPLE EQUATION SOLVING , 2014 .

[6]  James P. Byrnes,et al.  Explaining gender differences on SAT-math items. , 1993 .

[7]  Sarah-Jayne Blakemore,et al.  Imaging brain development: The adolescent brain , 2012, NeuroImage.

[8]  Adolf Af Ekenstam,et al.  A new approach to the assessment of children's mathematical competence , 1979 .

[9]  Robert Hecker,et al.  Sources of individual differences in IT , 1996 .

[10]  Yong-Ju Kwon,et al.  Linking Brain Growth with the Development of Scientific Reasoning Ability and Conceptual Change during Adolescence , 2000 .

[11]  A. Su,et al.  The National Council of Teachers of Mathematics , 1932, The Mathematical Gazette.

[12]  Kaye Stacey,et al.  STUDENTS' UNDERSTANDING OF ALGEBRAIC NOTATION: 11–15 , 1997 .

[13]  Steven A Hecht,et al.  Sources of individual differences in fraction skills. , 2003, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[14]  J. Piaget The Growth Of Logical Thinking From Childhood To Adolescence: An Essay On The Construction Of Formal Operational Structures , 1958 .

[15]  Carolyn Kieran Algebraic Thinking in the Early Grades: What Is It? , 2004 .

[16]  David C. Geary,et al.  Sexual selection and sex differences in mathematical abilities , 1996, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[17]  D. Schifter,et al.  A research companion to Principles and standards for school mathematics , 2003 .

[18]  Katharina Scheiter,et al.  The acquisition of problem-solving skills in mathematics: How animations can aid understanding of structural problem features and solution procedures , 2010 .

[19]  Janet Shibley Hyde,et al.  Cross-national patterns of gender differences in mathematics: a meta-analysis. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[20]  John R. Anderson,et al.  The change of the brain activation patterns as children learn algebra equation solving. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  R. Tennant Algebra , 1941, Nature.

[22]  Bracha Kramarski,et al.  Enhancing Mathematical Reasoning in the Classroom: The Effects of Cooperative Learning and Metacognitive Training , 2003 .

[23]  Isabell Wartenburger,et al.  Mathematical cognition: individual differences in resource allocation , 2010 .

[24]  Jean Piaget Piaget’s Theory , 1976 .

[25]  Edwin W. Schreiber The National Council of Teachers of Mathematics , 1936 .

[26]  John W. Renner,et al.  Relationships of science subject matter and developmental levels of learners , 1975 .

[27]  J. Piaget Intellectual Evolution from Adolescence to Adulthood , 1972 .

[28]  Janeen Lamb,et al.  Investigating functional thinking in the elementary classroom : foundations of early algebraic reasoning , 2006 .

[29]  James T. Townsend,et al.  Methods of Modeling Capacity in Simple Processing Systems , 2014 .

[30]  Elmar Cohors-Fresenborg,et al.  The role of metacognitive monitoring in explaining differences in mathematics achievement , 2010 .

[31]  R. H. Walters The Growth of Logical Thinking from Childhood to Adolescence , 1960 .

[32]  David W. Carraher,et al.  Arithmetic and Algebra in Early Mathematics Education , 2006 .

[33]  Kevin R. Gregg,et al.  SLA volume 17 issue 1 Cover and Front matter , 1995, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.

[34]  Jeremy Hodgen,et al.  Lower secondary school students' attitudes to mathematics: evidence from a large-scale survey in England , 2010 .