Technologically facilitated remoteness increases killing behavior

Abstract Technology now enables killing from remote locations. Killing remotely might be psychologically easier than killing face to face, which could promote more killing behavior and incur less severe emotional consequences. The current study manipulated the medium via which participants completed an ostensible ladybug-killing task. Participants who were in the same room as the insects killed fewer of them than participants who killed remotely via videoconference. Remoteness exerted an indirect effect on self-reported emotional consequences of killing. There was no additional effect of varying the ostensible location of the remote targets (same building vs. different state). This research emphasizes the importance of considering the psychological consequences of the remoteness technology affords.

[1]  Y. Trope,et al.  Construal-level theory of psychological distance. , 2010, Psychological review.

[2]  Anthony L. Cox,et al.  Post-combat invincibility: violent combat experiences are associated with increased risk-taking propensity following deployment. , 2008, Journal of psychiatric research.

[3]  Joel D. Lieberman,et al.  Terror management and aggression: evidence that mortality salience motivates aggression against worldview-threatening others. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[4]  S. Churchill,et al.  The Evolution of the Human Capacity for “Killing at a Distance”: The Human Fossil Evidence for the Evolution of ProjectileWeaponry , 2009 .

[5]  M. Coeckelbergh Drones, information technology, and distance: mapping the moral epistemology of remote fighting , 2013, Ethics and Information Technology.

[6]  Patrick Lin,et al.  Robot Ethics: The Ethical and Social Implications of Robotics , 2011 .

[7]  C. Marmar,et al.  The impact of reported direct and indirect killing on mental health symptoms in Iraq war veterans. , 2010, Journal of traumatic stress.

[8]  A. Martens,et al.  Evidence that Killing Escalates Within-Subjects in a Bug-Killing Paradigm. , 2011, Aggressive behavior.

[9]  Yaacov Trope,et al.  Politeness and psychological distance: a construal level perspective. , 2010, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  Yaacov Trope,et al.  Judging near and distant virtue and vice. , 2008, Journal of experimental social psychology.

[11]  S. Milgram Obedience to Authority: An Experimental View , 1975 .

[12]  Bryant J. Webber,et al.  Mental health diagnoses and counseling among pilots of remotely piloted aircraft in the United States Air Force. , 2013, MSMR.

[13]  M. Stein,et al.  Combat and peacekeeping operations in relation to prevalence of mental disorders and perceived need for mental health care: findings from a large representative sample of military personnel. , 2007, Archives of general psychiatry.

[14]  A. Martens,et al.  Evidence That Initial Obedient Killing Fuels Subsequent Volitional Killing Beyond Effects of Practice , 2010 .

[15]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  An fMRI Investigation of Emotional Engagement in Moral Judgment , 2001, Science.

[16]  Mary L. Cummings,et al.  Ethical and social issues in the design of weapon control computer interfaces , 2003, Proceedings. International Symposium on Technology and Society, 2003. Crime Prevention, Security and Design. 2003..

[17]  M. A. Safer,et al.  Killing versus witnessing in combat trauma and reports of PTSD symptoms and domestic violence. , 2011, Journal of traumatic stress.

[18]  Y. Trope,et al.  The effect of temporal distance on level of mental construal. , 2002 .

[19]  C. Marmar,et al.  Killing in combat, mental health symptoms, and suicidal ideation in Iraq war veterans. , 2011, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[20]  L. Williams,et al.  The Distinct Affective Consequences of Psychological Distance and Construal Level , 2014 .

[21]  J. Greenberg,et al.  Killing Begets Killing: Evidence From a Bug-Killing Paradigm That Initial Killing Fuels Subsequent Killing , 2007, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[22]  Patrick Lin,et al.  Killing Made Easy: From Joysticks to Politics , 2012 .

[23]  C. Dreu,et al.  Psychological distance boosts value-behavior correspondence in ultimatum bargaining and integrative negotiation , 2010 .

[24]  Laura E. Reardon,et al.  An analysis of post-traumatic stress symptoms in United States Air Force drone operators. , 2014, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[25]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Mapping the moral domain. , 2011, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[26]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[27]  N. Haslam,et al.  Meta-Milgram: An Empirical Synthesis of the Obedience Experiments , 2014, PloS one.