Controlling reaching movements during self-motion: body-fixed versus Earth-fixed targets.

The control of goal-directed arm movements performed during whole-body displacements is far from being understood. Recent studies suggested that the compensatory arm movements that allow individuals to preserve hand-in-space trajectory during unexpected body motion are controlled by sensorimotor, automatic- like processes. We tested this hypothesis comparing both the accuracy of movements directed towards body-fixed or Earth-fixed target during body rotations and the amount of interference of the reaching tasks on a concurrent cognitive task. Participants reached for a memorized 55 cm distant straight-ahead target in darkness which was about 20 cm lower than the initial finger position. The target was either body-fixed or Earth-fixed. At reaching onset, participants could be rotated in yaw. The concurrent task consisted of a verbal reaction time (RT) to an auditory stimulus. RTs increased when participants reached for the target while they were rotated. However, this increase was not significantly different for body-fixed and Earth-fixed targets. Reaching accuracy was greater for body-fixed than for Earth-fixed targets. A control experiment suggested that the errors in the Earth-fixed target condition arose from a difficulty in the organization of movements which necessitate both the production of active forces at the shoulder joint (to compensate for body rotation) and a concomitant decrease of muscular activation to lower the arm during reaching movements. These findings suggest that reaching for Earth-fixed or body-fixed targets during body rotation cannot be considered as being purely automatic tasks.

[1]  L Proteau,et al.  On the nature of the probe reaction-time task to uncover the attentional demands of movement. , 1984, Journal of motor behavior.

[2]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Proprioceptive control of cyclical bimanual forearm movements across different movement frequencies as revealed by means of tendon vibration , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  P Cordo,et al.  Proprioceptive coordination of movement sequences: role of velocity and position information. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  J. Lackner,et al.  Rapid adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectory. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  G. Spidalieri,et al.  Psychophysical properties of the trunk midline. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  J. Lackner,et al.  Gravitoinertial force background level affects adaptation to coriolis force perturbations of reaching movements. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  John C. Rothwell,et al.  What do reflex and voluntary mean? Modern views on an ancient debate , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Pointing movement in an artificial perturbing inertial field: A prospective paradigm for motor control study , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  Robert J. van Beers,et al.  How humans combine simultaneous proprioceptive and visual position information , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  I Israël,et al.  Vestibular information contributes to update retinotopic maps. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[11]  David A. Winter,et al.  Anticipatory control of center of mass and joint stability during voluntary arm movement from a standing posture: interplay between active and passive control , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  J. Lackner,et al.  Aspects of body self-calibration , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[13]  A. G. Feldman,et al.  Arm–trunk coordination in the absence of proprioception , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  K M Newell,et al.  Speed and accuracy of compensatory responses to limb disturbances. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  Paul DiZio,et al.  Sensorimotor aspects of high-speed artificial gravity: III. Sensorimotor adaptation. , 2002, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[16]  C. Bard,et al.  Production of short timing responses: a comparative study with a deafferented patient , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  Lucy Yardley,et al.  Attentional demands of continuously monitoring orientation using vestibular information , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  J. C. Rothwell,et al.  Postural electromyographic responses in the arm and leg following galvanic vestibular stimulation in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  B. Abernethy Dual-task methodology and motor skills research: Some applications and methodological constraints , 1988 .

[20]  Jean-Louis Vercher,et al.  On the nature of the vestibular control of arm-reaching movements during whole-body rotations , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  Christophe Bourdin,et al.  On-line versus off-line vestibular-evoked control of goal-directed arm movements , 2002, Neuroreport.

[22]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Vectorial coding of movement: vision, proprioception, or both? , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  C. Bourdin,et al.  Visual feedback of the moving arm allows complete adaptation of pointing movements to centrifugal and Coriolis forces in human subjects , 2001, Neuroscience Letters.

[24]  Christophe Bourdin,et al.  Vestibular signals contribute to the online control of goal-directed arm movements , 2002 .

[25]  M I Posner,et al.  Reduced attention and the performance of "automated" movements. , 1969, Journal of motor behavior.

[26]  J Blouin,et al.  Galvanic vestibular stimulation in humans produces online arm movement deviations when reaching towards memorized visual targets , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[27]  Sylvain Gagnon,et al.  Is egocentric space automatically encoded? , 2005, Acta psychologica.

[28]  C D Wickens,et al.  Age differences in the speed and capacity of information processing: 1. A dual-task approach. , 1987, Psychology and aging.

[29]  Jean-Louis Vercher,et al.  Fusion of visuo-ocular and vestibular signals in arm motor control. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  Anatol G. Feldman,et al.  Vestibular contribution to combined arm and trunk motion , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  Gabriel M. Gauthier,et al.  Encoding the position of a flashed visual target after passive body rotations , 1995, Neuroreport.

[32]  T. Pozzo,et al.  Hand trajectories of vertical arm movements in one-G and zero-G environments Evidence for a central representation of gravitational force , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  R. Chernikoff,et al.  Reaction time to kinesthetic stimulation resulting from sudden arm displacement. , 1952, Journal of experimental psychology.

[34]  Charalambos Papaxanthis,et al.  Effects of movement direction upon kinematic characteristics of vertical arm pointing movements in man , 1998, Neuroscience Letters.

[35]  T. Pozzo,et al.  Trajectories of arm pointing movements on the sagittal plane vary with both direction and speed , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  C. Bard,et al.  Aged-related differences in the attentional cost of pointing movements , 2003, Neuroscience Letters.

[37]  N. Teasdale,et al.  Attentional demands for postural control: the effects of aging and sensory reintegration. , 2001, Gait & posture.

[38]  N Lavie,et al.  Attentional demands of perception of passive self-motion in darkness , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  C. Bard,et al.  Attentional demands for static and dynamic equilibrium , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  J. Lackner,et al.  Coordinated turn-and-reach movements. I. Anticipatory compensation for self-generated coriolis and interaction torques. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  M J Carlton,et al.  Amending movements: the relationship between degree of mechanical disturbance and outcome accuracy. , 1983, Journal of motor behavior.

[42]  Lucy Yardley,et al.  Posture and mental task performance when viewing a moving visual field. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.