The Computational and Neural Basis of Rhythmic Timing in Medial Premotor Cortex

The neural underpinnings of rhythmic behavior, including music and dance, have been studied using the synchronization-continuation task (SCT), where subjects initially tap in synchrony with an isochronous metronome and then keep tapping at a similar rate via an internal beat mechanism. Here, we provide behavioral and neural evidence that supports a resetting drift-diffusion model (DDM) during SCT. Behaviorally, we show the model replicates the linear relation between the mean and standard-deviation of the intervals produced by monkeys in SCT. We then show that neural populations in the medial premotor cortex (MPC) contain an accurate trial-by-trial representation of elapsed-time between taps. Interestingly, the autocorrelation structure of the elapsed-time representation is consistent with a DDM. These results indicate that MPC has an orderly representation of time with features characteristic of concatenated DDMs and that this population signal can be used to orchestrate the rhythmic structure of the internally timed elements of SCT. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The present study used behavioral data, ensemble recordings from medial premotor cortex (MPC) in macaque monkeys, and computational modeling, to establish evidence in favor of a class of drift-diffusion models of rhythmic timing during a synchronization-continuation tapping task (SCT). The linear relation between the mean and standard-deviation of the intervals produced by monkeys in SCT is replicated by the model. Populations of MPC cells faithfully represent the elapsed time between taps, and there is significant trial-by-trial relation between decoded times and the timing behavior of the monkeys. Notably, the neural decoding properties, including its autocorrelation structure are consistent with a set of drift-diffusion models that are arranged sequentially and that are resetting in each SCT tap.

[1]  Hugo Merchant,et al.  Do we have a common mechanism for measuring time in the hundreds of millisecond range? Evidence from multiple-interval timing tasks. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  Patrick Simen,et al.  Scale (in)variance in a unified diffusion model of decision making and timing. , 2016, Psychological review.

[3]  Jay A. Hennig,et al.  Signal Multiplexing and Single-Neuron Computations in Lateral Intraparietal Area During Decision-Making , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  Ramon Bartolo,et al.  Dynamic Representation of the Temporal and Sequential Structure of Rhythmic Movements in the Primate Medial Premotor Cortex , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  Xiao-Jing Wang,et al.  The importance of mixed selectivity in complex cognitive tasks , 2013, Nature.

[6]  M. R. Jones,et al.  Dynamic attending and responses to time. , 1989, Psychological review.

[7]  J. Tanji,et al.  Interval time coding by neurons in the presupplementary and supplementary motor areas , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[8]  J. Tanji Sequential organization of multiple movements: involvement of cortical motor areas. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[9]  Hugo Merchant,et al.  Neural basis of the perception and estimation of time. , 2013, Annual review of neuroscience.

[10]  J. Tanji,et al.  The role of premotor cortex and the supplementary motor area in the temporal control of movement in man. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  Hugo Merchant,et al.  Subsecond timing in primates: comparison of interval production between human subjects and rhesus monkeys. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  P. A. Lewis,et al.  Brain activity correlates differentially with increasing temporal complexity of rhythms during initialisation, synchronisation, and continuation phases of paced finger tapping , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  Joachim Haß,et al.  A neurocomputational model for optimal temporal processing , 2008, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.

[14]  U. Karmarkar,et al.  Timing in the Absence of Clocks: Encoding Time in Neural Network States , 2007, Neuron.

[15]  R. Nichols,et al.  Prediction of the first year college performance of high aptitude students. , 1963, Psychological monographs.

[16]  Katsunori Kitano,et al.  Time representing cortical activities: two models inspired by prefrontal persistent activity , 2003, Biological Cybernetics.

[17]  Jonathan W. Pillow,et al.  Single-trial spike trains in parietal cortex reveal discrete steps during decision-making , 2015, Science.

[18]  Robert J. Zatorre,et al.  Interacting Cortical and Basal Ganglia Networks Underlying Finding and Tapping to the Musical Beat , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  Dean V Buonomano,et al.  Differential encoding of time by prefrontal and striatal network dynamics. , 2016, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[20]  Hugo Merchant,et al.  Motor system evolution and the emergence of high cognitive functions , 2014, Progress in Neurobiology.

[21]  Hugo Merchant,et al.  Measuring time with different neural chronometers during a synchronization-continuation task , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[22]  Christian K. Machens,et al.  Striatal dynamics explain duration judgments , 2015, bioRxiv.

[23]  A. Wing Voluntary Timing and Brain Function: An Information Processing Approach , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[24]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  A Model of Interval Timing by Neural Integration , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  Peter C. M. Molenaar,et al.  On the relation between the mean and the variance of a diffusion model response time distribution , 2005 .

[26]  D V Buonomano,et al.  Decoding Temporal Information: A Model Based on Short-Term Synaptic Plasticity , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  Henkjan Honing,et al.  Are non-human primates capable of rhythmic entrainment? Evidence for the gradual audiomotor evolution hypothesis , 2014, Front. Neurosci..

[28]  Robert E. Kass,et al.  Trial time warping to discriminate stimulus-related from movement-related neural activity , 2013, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[29]  M. Nicolelis,et al.  Interval timing and the encoding of signal duration by ensembles of cortical and striatal neurons. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.

[30]  G. Schöner Timing, Clocks, and Dynamical Systems , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[31]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Toward a neurobiology of temporal cognition: advances and challenges , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[32]  J. Hartigan,et al.  The Dip Test of Unimodality , 1985 .

[33]  Edward W. Large,et al.  Perceiving temporal regularity in music , 2002, Cogn. Sci..

[34]  A. Pouget,et al.  Variance as a Signature of Neural Computations during Decision Making , 2011, Neuron.

[35]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[36]  Athanasios Papoulis,et al.  Probability, Random Variables and Stochastic Processes , 1965 .

[37]  Bon-Mi Gu,et al.  Dedicated clock/timing-circuit theories of time perception and timed performance. , 2014, Advances in experimental medicine and biology.

[38]  J. Gibbon Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. , 1977 .

[39]  Michael N. Shadlen,et al.  A Neural Mechanism for Sensing and Reproducing a Time Interval , 2015, Current Biology.

[40]  Hugo Merchant,et al.  Interval Tuning in the Primate Medial Premotor Cortex as a General Timing Mechanism , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Response of Neurons in the Lateral Intraparietal Area during a Combined Visual Discrimination Reaction Time Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  Daniel Durstewitz,et al.  Time at the center, or time at the side? Assessing current models of time perception , 2016, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[43]  Matthew Brett,et al.  Rhythm and Beat Perception in Motor Areas of the Brain , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  Aniruddh D. Patel The Evolutionary Biology of Musical Rhythm: Was Darwin Wrong? , 2014, PLoS biology.

[45]  Hugo Merchant,et al.  Neurophysiology of timing in the hundreds of milliseconds: multiple layers of neuronal clocks in the medial premotor areas. , 2014, Advances in experimental medicine and biology.

[46]  Hugo Merchant,et al.  Finding the beat: a neural perspective across humans and non-human primates , 2015, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[47]  W. Meck,et al.  Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: coincidence detection of oscillatory processes. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[48]  J. Binder,et al.  Distributed Neural Systems Underlying the Timing of Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  Karen Cheng,et al.  Learning of Temporal Motor Patterns: An Analysis of Continuous Versus Reset Timing , 2011, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[50]  Hugo Merchant,et al.  Sensorimotor neural dynamics during isochronous tapping in the medial premotor cortex of the macaque , 2015, The European journal of neuroscience.

[51]  E. Large,et al.  Neural Networks for Beat Perception in Musical Rhythm , 2015, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[52]  E. Large Resonating to Musical Rhythm : Theory and Experiment , 2008 .

[53]  M. Treisman Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval. Implications for a model of the "internal clock". , 1963, Psychological monographs.