Winning a competition predicts dishonest behavior

Significance Competition is prevalent. People often resort to unethical means to win (e.g., the recent Volkswagen scandal). Not surprisingly, competition is central to the study of economics, psychology, sociology, political science, and more. Although we know much about contestants’ behavior before and during competitions, we know little about contestants’ behavior after the competition has ended. Connecting postcompetition behaviors with preceding competition experience, we find that after a competition is over winners behave more dishonestly than losers in an unrelated subsequent task. Furthermore, the subsequent unethical behavior effect seems to depend on winning, rather than on mere success. Providing insight into the issue is important in gaining understanding of how unethical behavior may cascade from exposure to competitive settings. Winning a competition engenders subsequent unrelated unethical behavior. Five studies reveal that after a competition has taken place winners behave more dishonestly than competition losers. Studies 1 and 2 demonstrate that winning a competition increases the likelihood of winners to steal money from their counterparts in a subsequent unrelated task. Studies 3a and 3b demonstrate that the effect holds only when winning means performing better than others (i.e., determined in reference to others) but not when success is determined by chance or in reference to a personal goal. Finally, study 4 demonstrates that a possible mechanism underlying the effect is an enhanced sense of entitlement among competition winners.

[1]  B. Major,et al.  From social inequality to personal entitlement: the role of social comparisons, legitimacy appraisals, and group membership , 1994 .

[2]  E. Fehr A Theory of Fairness, Competition and Cooperation , 1998 .

[3]  Lamar Pierce,et al.  Dishonesty in the Name of Equity , 2009, Psychological science.

[4]  B. Major,et al.  Social comparison processes and judgments of entitlement and satisfaction , 1989 .

[5]  Ilana Ritov,et al.  The role of actively open-minded thinking in information acquisition, accuracy, and calibration , 2013, Judgment and Decision Making.

[6]  Michel J. J. Handgraaf,et al.  Ethical Manoeuvring: Why People Avoid Both Major and Minor Lies , 2010 .

[7]  L. Treviño,et al.  Moral disengagement in ethical decision making: a study of antecedents and outcomes. , 2008, The Journal of applied psychology.

[8]  Max H. Bazerman,et al.  Blind Spots: Why We Fail to Do What's Right and What to Do about It , 2011 .

[9]  R. W. White Motivation reconsidered: the concept of competence. , 1959, Psychological review.

[10]  S. Harter Effectance Motivation Reconsidered. Toward a Developmental Model , 1978 .

[11]  T. Heatherton,et al.  Development and validation of a scale for measuring state self-esteem. , 1991 .

[12]  Nina Mazar,et al.  The Dishonesty of Honest People: A Theory of Self-Concept Maintenance , 2008 .

[13]  Maryam Kouchaki,et al.  Creative, Rare, Entitled, and Dishonest: How Commonality of Creativity in One’s Group Decreases an Individual’s Entitlement and Dishonesty , 2016 .

[14]  I. Ritov,et al.  Is that the answer you had in mind? The effect of perspective on unethical behavior , 2012, Judgment and Decision Making.

[15]  E. Rubaltelli,et al.  You can’t be better than me: The role of the reference point in modulating people’s pursuit of wealth , 2013 .

[16]  E. Fehr,et al.  Altruistic punishment in humans , 2002, Nature.

[17]  Yoella Bereby-Meyer,et al.  Honesty Requires Time (and Lack of Justifications) , 2012, Psychological science.

[18]  A. Bandura Moral Disengagement in the Perpetration of Inhumanities , 1999, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[19]  Michael W. Kraus,et al.  Class, chaos, and the construction of community. , 2012, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  L. Festinger A Theory of Social Comparison Processes , 1954 .

[21]  Shaul Shalvi Dishonestly increasing the likelihood of winning , 2012, Judgment and Decision Making.

[22]  Michel J. J. Handgraaf,et al.  Justified ethicality: Observing desired counterfactuals modifies ethical perceptions and behavior , 2011 .

[23]  Stéphane Côté,et al.  Higher social class predicts increased unethical behavior , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[24]  Uri Gneezy,et al.  Deception: The Role of Consequences , 2005 .

[25]  Leslie K. John,et al.  Cheating More for Less: Upward Social Comparisons Motivate the Poorly Compensated to Cheat , 2013 .

[26]  W. Keith Campbell,et al.  Psychological Entitlement: Interpersonal Consequences and Validation of a Self-Report Measure , 2004, Journal of personality assessment.

[27]  Lamar Pierce,et al.  The abundance effect: Unethical behavior in the presence of wealth , 2009 .

[28]  Stephen M. Garcia,et al.  The Psychology of Competition , 2013, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[29]  Nina Mazar,et al.  Commentaries and Rejoinder to “The Dishonesty of Honest People” , 2008 .

[30]  Michel J. J. Handgraaf,et al.  People avoid situations that enable them to deceive others , 2011 .

[31]  Shaul Shalvi,et al.  The collaborative roots of corruption , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[32]  Michael W. Kraus,et al.  Having less, giving more: the influence of social class on prosocial behavior. , 2010, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[33]  B. Monin,et al.  Victim entitlement to behave selfishly. , 2010, Journal of personality and social psychology.