A nonmusical paradigm for identifying absolute pitch possessors.

The ability to identify and reproduce sounds of specific frequencies is remarkable and uncommon. The etiology and defining characteristics of this skill, absolute pitch (AP), have been very controversial. One theory suggests that AP requires a specific type of early musical training and that the ability to encode and remember tones depends on these learned musical associations. An alternate theory argues that AP may be strongly dependent on hereditary factors and relatively independent of musical experience. To date, it has been difficult to test these hypotheses because all previous paradigms for identifying AP have required subjects to employ knowledge of musical nomenclature. As such, these tests are insensitive to the possibility of discovering AP in either nonmusicians or musicians of non-Western training. Based on previous literature in pitch memory, a paradigm is presented that is intended to distinguish between AP possessors and nonpossessors independent of the subjects' musical experience. The efficacy of this method is then tested with 20 classically defined AP possessors and 22 nonpossessors. Data from these groups strongly support the validity of the paradigm. The use of a nonmusical paradigm to identify AP may facilitate research into many aspects of this phenomenon.

[1]  L. Demany,et al.  Dissociation of pitch from timbre in auditory short-term memory. , 1991, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[2]  D. Deutsch Tones and Numbers: Specificity of Interference in Immediate Memory , 1970, Science.

[3]  P. Gregersen,et al.  Early childhood music education and predisposition to absolute pitch: teasing apart genes and environment. , 2001, American journal of medical genetics.

[4]  D Deutsch,et al.  Mapping of Interactions in the Pitch Memory Store , 1972, Science.

[5]  B. Delgutte,et al.  A possible neurophysiological basis of the octave enlargement effect. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[6]  John C Gore,et al.  Absolute Pitch Does Not Depend on Early Musical Training , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[7]  E. Donchin,et al.  People with Absolute Pitch Process Tones Without Producing a P300 , 1984, Science.

[8]  P. Vernon Absolute pitch: a case study. , 1977, British journal of psychology.

[9]  R J Zatorre,et al.  Multiple coding strategies in the retention of musical tones by possessors of absolute pitch , 1989, Memory & cognition.

[10]  A. Halpern,et al.  Memory for the absolute pitch of familiar songs , 1989, Memory & cognition.

[11]  Lola L. Cuddy,et al.  Training the absolute identification of pitch , 1970 .

[12]  Stewart H. Hulse,et al.  Absolute-Pitch Judgments of Black and White-Key Pitches , 1991 .

[13]  A. Bachem,et al.  Time Factors in Relative and Absolute Pitch Determination , 1954 .

[14]  P. Gregersen,et al.  Instant recognition: the genetics of pitch perception. , 1998, American journal of human genetics.

[15]  R. Patterson,et al.  Analyzing Pitch Chroma and Pitch Height in the Human Brain , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[16]  P. Gregersen,et al.  Absolute pitch: prevalence, ethnic variation, and estimation of the genetic component. , 1999, American journal of human genetics.

[17]  T. Pechmann,et al.  Interference in memory for tonal pitch: Implications for a working-memory model , 1992, Memory & cognition.

[18]  A. Bachem,et al.  Various Types of Absolute Pitch , 1937 .

[19]  Peter Cariani,et al.  Temporal Codes, Timing Nets, and Music Perception , 2001 .

[20]  E. Schellenberg,et al.  Good Pitch Memory Is Widespread , 2003, Psychological science.

[21]  D. Levitin Absolute memory for musical pitch: Evidence from the production of learned melodies , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  V. T. Wynn Absolute pitch in humans, its variations and possible connections with other known rhythmic phenomena. , 1973, Progress in neurobiology.

[23]  K. Miyazaki Musical pitch identification by absolute pitch possessors , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[24]  R. Frisina,et al.  Neural processing of musical timbre by musicians, nonmusicians, and musicians possessing absolute pitch. , 1994, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[25]  D. Sergeant,et al.  Experimental Investigation of Absolute Pitch , 1969 .

[26]  E C Hantz,et al.  Absolute pitch and sex effect event-related potential activity for a melodic interval discrimination task. , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[27]  N. Freimer,et al.  Absolute pitch: an approach for identification of genetic and nongenetic components. , 1998, American journal of human genetics.

[28]  K. Ohgushi,et al.  The origin of tonality and a possible explanation of the octave enlargement phenomenon. , 1983, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[29]  N. Freimer,et al.  Familial aggregation of absolute pitch. , 2000, American journal of human genetics.

[30]  V. Chaloupka,et al.  Observation of a reversible, medication-induced change in pitch perception. , 1994, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[31]  Sensory and verbal coding strategies in subjects with absolute pitch. , 1974 .

[32]  N Cowan,et al.  Can auditory memory for tone pitch be rehearsed? , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.