Performance evaluation judgments: Effects of prior experience under different performance evaluation schemes and feedback frequencies

This paper documents that previous experience under a performance evaluation system can systematically bias decision makers' subsequent evaluations. In the experiment reported in this paper, subjects made a series of investment decisions under one of four accounting systems, created by crossing evaluation scheme (outcome-based versus decision-based) with outcome feedback frequency (frequent versus infrequent), and then evaluated the performance of other decision makers who had made similar decisions. Consistent with prior research (e.g., Brown and Solomon [1987; 1993], Fisher and Selling [1993], Kennedy [1995], Lipe [1993], and Tan and Lipe [1997]), the evaluations were affected by knowledge of outcomes (i.e., outcome effect). More importantly, the size of this outcome effect varied across the four accounting systems, consistent with an interaction between evaluation scheme and outcome feedback frequency. As expected, experience under the outcome-based evaluation scheme caused a larger outcome effect than experience under the decision-based scheme, and frequent outcome feedback increased the difference between the two schemes.

[1]  Hun-Tong Tan,et al.  Outcome effects: The impact of decision process and outcome controllability , 1997 .

[2]  Richard M. Tubbs,et al.  The effect of experience on the auditor's organization and amount of knowledge , 1988 .

[3]  Hun-Tong Tan,et al.  Effects Of Expectations, Prior Involvement, And Review Awareness On Memory For Audit Evidence And Judgment , 1995 .

[4]  Kimberley J Smith Investment monitoring systems, abandonment of capital assets, and firm performance , 1993 .

[5]  Jane Kennedy,et al.  Debiasing the curse of knowledge in audit judgment , 1995 .

[6]  Jonathan Baron,et al.  Judgment by outcomes: When is it justified? , 1992 .

[7]  M. L. Schroth Effects of frequency of feedback on transfer in concept identification. , 1997, The American journal of psychology.

[8]  Madhav V. Rajan,et al.  Cost Accounting: A Managerial Emphasis , 1972 .

[9]  R. Glaser Education and Thinking: The Role of Knowledge. , 1984 .

[10]  S. Young PARTICIPATIVE BUDGETING - THE EFFECTS OF RISK-AVERSION AND ASYMMETRIC INFORMATION ON BUDGETARY SLACK , 1985 .

[11]  Clifton E. Brown,et al.  An Experimental Investigation of Explanations for Outcome Effects on Appraisals of Capital‐Budgeting Decisions* , 1993 .

[12]  P. Johnson-Laird,et al.  Spatial Descriptions and Referential Continuity , 1982 .

[13]  William F. Brewer,et al.  Memory for goal-directed events , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[14]  M. W. Nelson,et al.  Knowledge structure and the estimation of conditional probabilities in audit planning , 1995 .

[15]  M. J. Houston,et al.  Goal-Oriented Experiences and the Development of Knowledge , 1993 .

[16]  Marlys Gascho Lipe,et al.  Analyzing the variance investigation decision: The effects of outcomes, mental accounting, and , 1993 .

[17]  Allen Newell,et al.  Human Problem Solving. , 1973 .

[18]  Joan L. Luft,et al.  Bonus and penalty incentives contract choice by employees , 1994 .

[19]  W. Waller,et al.  The auditor and learning from experience: Some conjectures , 1984 .

[20]  R. Kirk Experimental Design: Procedures for the Behavioral Sciences , 1970 .

[21]  Richard A. Schmidt,et al.  The learning of generalized motor programs: Reducing the relative frequency of knowledge of results enhances memory. , 1989 .

[22]  R. Hogarth,et al.  Confidence in judgment: Persistence of the illusion of validity. , 1978 .

[23]  Peter F. Luckett,et al.  Feedback and management accounting: A review of research into behavioural consequences , 1991 .

[24]  D. Medin,et al.  The role of theories in conceptual coherence. , 1985, Psychological review.