Effects of aging, Parkinson's disease, and dopaminergic medication on response selection and control

We examined effects of short-term and long-term dopaminergic medication in Parkinson's disease on conflict monitoring or response selection processes. These processes were examined using event-related potentials (ERPs), while subjects performed a stimulus-response (S-R) compatibility task. An extended sample of young and elderly controls, Parkinson's disease patients with a medication history (PDs) and initially diagnosed, drug-naïve de novo PD patients (de novo PDs) were enrolled. Both PD groups were measured twice (on and off-medication or before and 8 weeks after medication onset). The results show that dopaminergic intervention selectively reduced the pathologically enhanced response selection in compatible S-R relations. This medication effect was already evident after short-term treatment, not differing from long-term treatment and performance in elderly controls. Contrary, age-related attenuations of the N2 in incompatible S-R relations, probably reflecting impaired conflict processing or response control, are unaffected by medication. The results suggest that compatible and incompatible S-R relations demand different neuronal mechanisms within the basal ganglia, as only the former are affected by agonizing the dopaminergic system.

[1]  J. Stout,et al.  Activation of conflicting responses in Parkinson's disease: evidence for degrading and facilitating effects on response time , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  P. Redgrave,et al.  The basal ganglia: a vertebrate solution to the selection problem? , 1999, Neuroscience.

[3]  L. Nyberg,et al.  The correlative triad among aging, dopamine, and cognition: Current status and future prospects , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[4]  C. Doucet,et al.  The effect of response execution on P3 latency, reaction time, and movement time. , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[5]  E. Morris,et al.  Age-related decline in striatal volume in monkeys as measured by magnetic resonance imaging , 2000, Neurobiology of Aging.

[6]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  Flanker interference in young and older participants as reflected in event-related potentials , 2008, Brain Research.

[7]  G. Bernardi,et al.  Age-related functional changes of high-voltage-activated calcium channels in different neuronal subtypes of mouse striatum , 2008, Neuroscience.

[8]  W. Dauer,et al.  Parkinson's Disease Mechanisms and Models , 2003, Neuron.

[9]  A. Antal,et al.  Visuo-cognitive dysfunctions in Parkinson's disease. , 1998, Clinical neuroscience.

[10]  H. Bergman,et al.  Information processing, dimensionality reduction and reinforcement learning in the basal ganglia , 2003, Progress in Neurobiology.

[11]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  ERP—Correlates of response selection in a response conflict paradigm , 2008, Brain Research.

[12]  B. Fischl,et al.  Cognitive function, P3a/P3b brain potentials, and cortical thickness in aging , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[13]  M. Westerfield,et al.  Modality-specificity of sensory aging in vision and audition: Evidence from event-related potentials , 2008, Brain Research.

[14]  T. Robbins,et al.  Functions of frontostriatal systems in cognition: Comparative neuropsychopharmacological studies in rats, monkeys and humans , 2006, Biological Psychology.

[15]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Effects of stimulus-response compatibility in Parkinson’s disease: a psychophysiological analysis , 2006, Journal of Neural Transmission.

[16]  D. Surmeier,et al.  D1 and D2 dopamine-receptor modulation of striatal glutamatergic signaling in striatal medium spiny neurons , 2007, Trends in Neurosciences.

[17]  C. Carter,et al.  The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP studies , 2002, Physiology & Behavior.

[18]  P. Praamstra,et al.  Failed Suppression of Direct Visuomotor Activation in Parkinson's Disease , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  Stimulus-response compatibility in Huntington's disease: a cognitive-neurophysiological analysis. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  W. Sommer,et al.  Postperceptual effects and P300 latency. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[21]  S. Berman,et al.  P300 development during adolescence: Effects of DRD2 genotype , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[22]  J. Wickens,et al.  Computational models of the basal ganglia: from robots to membranes , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.

[23]  D F Stegeman,et al.  Reliance on external cues for movement initiation in Parkinson's disease. Evidence from movement-related potentials. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[24]  T. Chase,et al.  Striatal dopamine- and glutamate-mediated dysregulation in experimental parkinsonism , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[25]  Ulrich Hegerl,et al.  Neurochemical Substrates and Neuroanatomical Generators of the Event-Related P300 , 1999, Neuropsychobiology.

[26]  B. Kopp,et al.  N2, P3 and the lateralized readiness potential in a nogo task involving selective response priming. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[27]  M. Falkenstein,et al.  Error Processing in Huntington's Disease , 2006, PloS one.

[28]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  A. Kok Age-related changes in involuntary and voluntary attention as reflected in components of the event-related potential (ERP) , 2000, Biological Psychology.