Threats, rewards, and attention deployment in anxious youth and adults: An eye tracking study

The current study examines anxiety and age associations with attention allocation and physiological response to threats and rewards. Twenty-two healthy-adults, 20 anxious-adults, 26 healthy-youth, and 19 anxious-youth completed two eye-tracking tasks. In the Visual Scene Task (VST), participants' fixations were recorded while they viewed a central neutral image flanked by two threatening or two rewarding stimuli. In the Negative Words Task (NWT), physiological response was measured by means of pupil diameter change while negative and neutral words were presented. For both tasks, no interaction was found between anxiety and age-group. In the VST, anxious participants avoided the threatening images when groups were collapsed across age. Similarly, adults but not adolescents avoided the threatening images when collapsed across anxiety. No differences were found for rewarding images. In NWT, all subjects demonstrated increase in pupil dilation after word presentation. Only main effect of age emerged with stronger pupil dilation in adults than children. Finally, maximum pupil change was correlated with threat avoidance bias in the scene task. Gaze patterns and pupil dilation show that anxiety and age are associated with attention allocation to threats. The relations between attention and autonomic arousal point to a complex interaction between bottom-up and top-down processes as they relate to attention allocation.

[1]  B. Luna,et al.  What has fMRI told us about the Development of Cognitive Control through Adolescence? , 2010, Brain and Cognition.

[2]  M. Wessa,et al.  Regulating the blink: Cognitive reappraisal modulates attention , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[3]  Y. Bar-Haim Research review: Attention bias modification (ABM): a novel treatment for anxiety disorders. , 2010, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[4]  K. Mogg,et al.  A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. , 1998, Behaviour research and therapy.

[5]  E. Leibenluft,et al.  ATTENTION BIAS OF ANXIOUS YOUTH DURING EXTENDED EXPOSURE OF EMOTIONAL FACE PAIRS: AN EYE‐TRACKING STUDY , 2013, Depression and anxiety.

[6]  E. Knudsen Fundamental components of attention. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[7]  Dima Amso,et al.  The attentive brain: insights from developmental cognitive neuroscience , 2015, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[8]  T. Paus Mapping brain development and aggression. , 2005, The Canadian child and adolescent psychiatry review = La revue canadienne de psychiatrie de l'enfant et de l'adolescent.

[9]  Mike Rinck,et al.  Spider fearful individuals attend to threat, then quickly avoid it: evidence from eye movements. , 2006, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[10]  Joe Kossowsky,et al.  Vigilance and Avoidance of Threat in the Eye Movements of Children with Separation Anxiety Disorder , 2010, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[11]  J. Gross Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[12]  Joseph E LeDoux Emotional networks and motor control: a fearful view. , 1996, Progress in brain research.

[13]  Sarah-Jayne Blakemore,et al.  Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? , 2014, Annual review of psychology.

[14]  Greg J Siegle,et al.  Pupillary reactivity to emotional information in child and adolescent depression: links to clinical and ecological measures. , 2007, The American journal of psychiatry.

[15]  Tim Dalgleish,et al.  The development of a corpus of emotional words produced by children and adolescents , 1999 .

[16]  Thomas F. Nugent,et al.  Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[17]  J. Beatty Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. , 1982 .

[18]  R. Rapee,et al.  The time-course of attentional bias in anxious children and adolescents. , 2009, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[19]  D. Pine,et al.  The time course of attentional bias for emotional faces in anxious children , 2010 .

[20]  K. Mogg,et al.  Time course of attentional bias for threat information in non-clinical anxiety. , 1997, Behaviour research and therapy.

[21]  B. Tuschen-Caffier,et al.  Hypervigilance and avoidance in visual attention in children with social phobia. , 2014, Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry.

[22]  E. Granholm,et al.  Pupillary responses index cognitive resource limitations. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[23]  Greg J Siegle,et al.  Use of concurrent pupil dilation assessment to inform interpretation and analysis of fMRI data , 2003, NeuroImage.

[24]  Y. Bar-Haim,et al.  Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.

[25]  C. Frith,et al.  Adolescent development of the neural circuitry for thinking about intentions , 2007, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[26]  Eveline A. Crone,et al.  Executive functions in adolescence: inferences from brain and behavior. , 2009, Developmental science.

[27]  Guy M. Goodwin,et al.  Lateral Prefrontal Cortex Mediates the Cognitive Modification of Attentional Bias , 2010, Biological Psychiatry.

[28]  C. MacLeod,et al.  Attentional bias in emotional disorders. , 1986, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[29]  E. Leibenluft,et al.  The social re-orientation of adolescence: a neuroscience perspective on the process and its relation to psychopathology , 2005, Psychological Medicine.

[30]  Monique Ernst,et al.  Attention biases, anxiety, and development: toward or away from threats or rewards? , 2012, Depression and anxiety.

[31]  Eric Granholm,et al.  Pupillary and reaction time measures of sustained processing of negative information in depression , 2001, Biological Psychiatry.

[32]  Greg J. Siegle,et al.  Remission Prognosis for Cognitive Therapy for Recurrent Depression Using the Pupil: Utility and Neural Correlates , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.

[33]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[34]  Cameron S. Carter,et al.  Do the Seconds Turn Into Hours? Relationships between Sustained Pupil Dilation in Response to Emotional Information and Self-Reported Rumination , 2003, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[35]  O. John,et al.  Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: personality processes, individual differences, and life span development. , 2004, Journal of personality.

[36]  J. Gross,et al.  The cognitive control of emotion , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[37]  F. Blanchard-Fields,et al.  The experience of anger and sadness in everyday problems impacts age differences in emotion regulation. , 2008, Developmental psychology.