Attentional responses on an auditory oddball predict false memory susceptibility
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] A. Kok,et al. The effect of repetition of infrequent familiar and unfamiliar visual patterns on components of the event-related brain potential , 1980, Biological Psychology.
[2] John E. Kiat,et al. Assessing cross-modal target transition effects with a visual-auditory oddball. , 2018, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[3] Neil A. Macmillan,et al. Detection theory: A user's guide, 2nd ed. , 2005 .
[4] A. Kok. On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. , 2001, Psychophysiology.
[5] Myra A. Fernandes,et al. Illusory recollection in older adults and younger adults under divided attention. , 2009, Psychology and aging.
[6] A. Fernández-Bouzas,et al. Clinical correlations of grey matter reductions in the caudate nucleus of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. , 2010, Journal of psychiatry & neuroscience : JPN.
[7] B. Lütkenhöner. Dipole source localization by means of maximum likelihood estimation. II. Experimental evaluation. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[8] M. Scherg,et al. Localizing P300 Generators in Visual Target and Distractor Processing: A Combined Event-Related Potential and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[9] Jeremy C. Rietschel,et al. Evidence for a new late positive ERP component in an attended novelty oddball task. , 2010, Psychophysiology.
[10] Daniel L Schacter,et al. Not all false memories are created equal: the neural basis of false recognition. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.
[11] Jessica A. Grahn,et al. The role of the basal ganglia in learning and memory: Neuropsychological studies , 2009, Behavioural Brain Research.
[12] L. Garcia-Larrea,et al. P3, positive slow wave and working memory load: a study on the functional correlates of slow wave activity. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[13] P. Derambure,et al. Role of Basal Ganglia Circuits in Resisting Interference by Distracters: A swLORETA Study , 2012, PloS one.
[14] E. Golob,et al. Cortical potentials in an auditory oddball task reflect individual differences in working memory capacity. , 2013, Psychophysiology.
[15] J. Polich,et al. Normative Variation of P3a and P3b from a Large Sample , 2007 .
[16] T W Picton,et al. Temporal and sequential probability in evoked potential studies. , 1981, Canadian journal of psychology.
[17] A. Nobre,et al. Long-term memory prepares neural activity for perception , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[18] J. Rosenfeld,et al. Event-related potentials in the dual task paradigm: P300 discriminates engaging and non-engaging films when film-viewing is the primary task. , 1992, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[19] Sean M. Lane,et al. Dividing attention during a witnessed event increases eyewitness suggestibility , 2006 .
[20] W Ritter,et al. A Review of Event‐Related Potential Components Discovered in the Context of Studying P3 a , 1992, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[21] Astrid M. Schloerscheidt,et al. Creating memory illusions: Expectancy-based processing and the generation of false memories , 2002, Memory.
[22] M. Kane,et al. Dealing With Prospective Memory Demands While Performing an Ongoing Task: Shared Processing, Increased On-Task Focus, or Both? , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[23] M. Rivardo,et al. Integrating Inattentional Blindness and Eyewitness Memory , 2011 .
[24] Vince D Calhoun,et al. fMRI in an oddball task: effects of target-to-target interval. , 2005, Psychophysiology.
[25] N. Squires,et al. Two varieties of long-latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[26] Sander Nieuwenhuis,et al. Noradrenergic and cholinergic modulation of late ERP responses to deviant stimuli. , 2015, Psychophysiology.
[27] R. Poldrack,et al. Dissociable Controlled Retrieval and Generalized Selection Mechanisms in Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex , 2005, Neuron.
[28] J. Cohen,et al. On the number of trials needed for P300. , 1997, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[29] H L Roediger,et al. Imagination inflation for action events: Repeated imaginings lead to illusory recollections , 1998, Memory & cognition.
[30] Bettina Sorger,et al. Novelty and target processing during an auditory novelty oddball: A simultaneous event-related potential and functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 2008, NeuroImage.
[31] Michael D. Rugg,et al. Effects of Divided Attention on fMRI Correlates of Memory Encoding , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[32] Yuezhi Li,et al. Localizing P300 generators in high-density event- related potential with fMRI. , 2009, Medical science monitor : international medical journal of experimental and clinical research.
[33] E. Donchin. Presidential address, 1980. Surprise!...Surprise? , 1981, Psychophysiology.
[34] Qualities of the unreal. , 1986 .
[35] Godfrey Pearlson,et al. An adaptive reflexive processing model of neurocognitive function: supporting evidence from a large scale (n = 100) fMRI study of an auditory oddball task , 2005, NeuroImage.
[36] E. Donchin,et al. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[37] M. Corbetta,et al. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[38] Lorraine Hope,et al. The effects of divided attention at study and reporting procedure on regulation and monitoring for episodic recall. , 2016, Acta psychologica.
[39] D. S. Lindsay,et al. Memory impairment and source misattribution in postevent misinformation experiments with short retention intervals , 1994, Memory & cognition.
[40] Nicholas B. Turk-Browne,et al. Memory-guided attention: control from multiple memory systems , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[41] C. Frith,et al. Monitoring for target objects: activation of right frontal and parietal cortices with increasing time on task , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[42] Daniel S. Ruchkin,et al. 11 Positive Slow Wave and P300: Association and Disassociation , 1983 .
[43] Roberto Hornero,et al. Auditory P3a and P3b neural generators in schizophrenia: An adaptive sLORETA P300 localization approach , 2015, Schizophrenia Research.
[44] Matthew A. Palmer,et al. Phenomenological reports diagnose accuracy of eyewitness identification decisions. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[45] John Polich,et al. Affective ERP processing in a visual oddball task: Arousal, valence, and gender , 2008, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[46] C. Stark,et al. Neural activity during encoding predicts false memories created by misinformation. , 2005, Learning & memory.
[47] I. Gotlib,et al. An attentional scope model of rumination. , 2013, Psychological bulletin.
[48] R. C. Oldfield. THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .
[49] F Rösler,et al. Toward a functional categorization of slow waves: taking into account past and future events. , 1991, Psychophysiology.
[50] Hongkeun Kim. Involvement of the dorsal and ventral attention networks in oddball stimulus processing: A meta‐analysis , 2014, Human brain mapping.
[51] K. Kiehl,et al. An event-related fMRI study of visual and auditory oddball tasks , 2001 .
[52] N. Turk-Browne,et al. How Hippocampal Memory Shapes, and Is Shaped by, Attention , 2017 .
[53] H. Blank,et al. How to protect eyewitness memory against the misinformation effect: A meta-analysis of post-warning studies , 2014 .
[54] H. Gray,et al. P300 as an index of attention to self-relevant stimuli , 2004 .
[55] Robert J. Barry,et al. Reinstating the Novelty P3 , 2016, Scientific Reports.
[56] R. Belli,et al. An exploratory high-density EEG investigation of the misinformation effect: Attentional and recollective differences between true and false perceptual memories , 2017, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.
[57] Myra A. Fernandes,et al. Divided attention and memory: evidence of substantial interference effects at retrieval and encoding. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[58] Joseph Dien,et al. The ERP PCA Toolkit: An open source program for advanced statistical analysis of event-related potential data , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[59] M. L. Kietzman,et al. Slow wave and P300 in signal detection. , 1980, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[60] Jocelyn Parong,et al. The misinformation effect is unrelated to the DRM effect with and without a DRM warning , 2016, Memory.
[61] Robert Oostenveld,et al. FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data , 2010, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..
[62] H. Engeland,et al. Event-related potentials and performance of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Children and normal controls in auditory and visual selective attention tasks , 1997, Biological Psychiatry.
[63] C. Gonsalvez,et al. Can working memory predict target-to-target interval effects in the P300? , 2013, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[64] Neil A. Macmillan,et al. Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .
[65] M. Posner,et al. The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. , 2012, Annual review of neuroscience.
[66] R Näätänen,et al. Frontal negative and parietal positive components of the slow wave dissociated. , 1987, Psychophysiology.
[67] B. Lutkenhoner,et al. Dipole source localization by means of maximum likelihood estimation I. Theory and simulations. , 1998 .
[68] I Rosén,et al. Memory for perceived and imagined pictures—an event-related potential study , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[69] Maurizio Corbetta,et al. Large-scale brain networks account for sustained and transient activity during target detection , 2009, NeuroImage.
[70] D. Rujescu,et al. The neural basis of the P300 potential , 2004, European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience.
[71] M. Corbetta,et al. Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[72] Rainer Goebel,et al. Localizing P300 Generators in Visual Target and Distractor Processing: A Combined Event-Related Potential and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[73] M. Zaragoza,et al. The Misinformation Effect , 2012 .
[74] J. Polich. Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[75] E. Loftus. Our changeable memories: legal and practical implications , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[76] Daniel Strüber,et al. P300 and slow wave from oddball and single-stimulus visual tasks: inter-stimulus interval effects. , 2002, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[77] A. Coenen,et al. Neural generators of the auditory evoked potential components P3a and P3b. , 2012, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.
[78] Mario F. Mendez,et al. Neurobehavioral changes associated with caudate lesions , 1989, Neurology.
[79] Beatrice G. Kuhlmann,et al. Influences of Source - Item Contingency and Schematic Knowledge on Source Monitoring: Tests of the Probability-Matching Account. , 2011, Journal of memory and language.
[80] Jessica A. Grahn,et al. The cognitive functions of the caudate nucleus , 2008, Progress in Neurobiology.
[81] M. Mesulam,et al. Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events. , 1999, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[82] M. Posner,et al. The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.
[83] Christopher Barry,et al. The effect of divided attention on false memory depends on how memory is tested , 2007, Memory & cognition.
[84] I. Robertson,et al. `Oops!': Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects , 1997, Neuropsychologia.
[85] E. Donchin,et al. Spatiotemporal analysis of the late ERP responses to deviant stimuli. , 2001, Psychophysiology.
[86] Nash Unsworth,et al. The influence of lapses of attention on working memory capacity , 2016, Memory & cognition.
[87] S. Lane,et al. Processing resources and eyewitness suggestibility , 1998 .
[88] G. Murphy,et al. Perceptual Load Affects Eyewitness Accuracy and Susceptibility to Leading Questions , 2016, Front. Psychol..
[89] E F Loftus,et al. Discrepancy detection and vulnerability to misleading postevent information , 1986, Memory & cognition.
[90] E. Stein,et al. Multiple Neuronal Networks Mediate Sustained Attention , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[91] Michael C. Anderson,et al. Suppressing Unwanted Memories , 2009 .
[92] Mara Mather,et al. (www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/acp.857 Source Monitoring and Suggestibility to Misinformation: Adult Age-Related Differences , 2022 .
[93] E Donchin,et al. Second Thoughts : Multiple P 3 OOs Elicited by a Single Stimulus , 2005 .
[94] A. Engel,et al. What is novel in the novelty oddball paradigm? Functional significance of the novelty P3 event-related potential as revealed by independent component analysis. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[95] E. John,et al. Evoked-Potential Correlates of Stimulus Uncertainty , 1965, Science.
[96] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[97] R. Benson,et al. Responses to rare visual target and distractor stimuli using event-related fMRI. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.
[98] P. Higham. Believing details known to have been suggested , 1998 .
[99] S. Segalowitz,et al. Source Monitoring: ERP Evidence for Greater Reactivity to Nontarget Information in Older Adults , 1998, Brain and Cognition.
[100] Hunter G. Hoffman,et al. Role of memory strength in reality monitoring decisions: evidence from source attribution biases. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[101] Vincenzo Varriale,et al. Impulsivity, intelligence and P300 wave: an empirical study. , 2008, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[102] M. Brand,et al. Observation Inflation , 2010, Psychological science.
[103] P. Sajda,et al. Simultaneous EEG-fMRI Reveals Temporal Evolution of Coupling between Supramodal Cortical Attention Networks and the Brainstem , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[104] Vinod Menon,et al. Where and When the Anterior Cingulate Cortex Modulates Attentional Response: Combined fMRI and ERP Evidence , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[105] H Pratt,et al. P300 in response to the subject's own name. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[106] André Beauducel,et al. Biopsychological foundations of extraversion differential effort reactivity and state control , 1997 .
[107] S. Galderisi,et al. The cortical generators of P3a and P3b: A LORETA study , 2007, Brain Research Bulletin.
[108] E. Gordon,et al. THE TEST-RETEST RELIABILITY OF A STANDARDIZED NEUROCOGNITIVE AND NEUROPHYSIOLOGICAL TEST BATTERY: “NEUROMARKER” , 2005, The International journal of neuroscience.
[109] R. J. Doherty,et al. Separation of the components of the late positive complex in an ERP dishabituation paradigm , 2005, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[110] L. McEvoy,et al. High resolution evoked potential imaging of the cortical dynamics of human working memory. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[111] Edward F. Jackson,et al. Caudate Nucleus Volume Asymmetry Predicts Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Symptomatology in Children , 2002, Journal of child neurology.
[112] Arnaud Delorme,et al. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[113] Mateusz Polak,et al. Toward a Non-memory Misinformation Effect: Accessing the Original Source Does Not Prevent Yielding to Misinformation , 2016 .
[114] H. Blank,et al. Effects of Postwarning Specificity on Memory Performance and Confidence in the Eyewitness Misinformation Paradigm , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.
[115] M. Chun,et al. Interactions between attention and memory , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[116] Joseph Dien,et al. Evaluating two-step PCA of ERP data with Geomin, Infomax, Oblimin, Promax, and Varimax rotations. , 2010, Psychophysiology.
[117] P. Fox,et al. Functional Decoding and Meta-analytic Connectivity Modeling in Adult Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder , 2016, Biological Psychiatry.
[118] J. Read,et al. Effects of divided attention and word concreteness on correct recall and false memory reports , 2002, Memory.
[119] Hong Li,et al. Temporal features of the degree effect in self-relevance: Neural correlates , 2011, Biological Psychology.
[120] Role of memory strength in reality monitoring decisions: evidence from source attribution biases. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[121] Michael C. Anderson,et al. Suppressing unwanted memories by executive control , 2001, Nature.
[122] K. McDermott,et al. Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. , 1995 .