Attentional responses on an auditory oddball predict false memory susceptibility

Attention and memory are highly integrated processes. Building on prior behavioral investigations, this study assesses the link between individual differences in low-level neural attentional responding and false memory susceptibility on the misinformation effect, a paradigm in which false event memories are induced via misleading post-event information. Twenty-four subjects completed the misinformation effect paradigm after which high-density (256-channel) EEG data was collected as they engaged in an auditory oddball task. Temporal-spatial decomposition was used to extract two attention-related components from the oddball data, the P3b and Classic Slow Wave. The P3b was utilized as an index of individual differences in salient target attentional responding while the slow wave was adopted as an index of variability in task-level sustained attention. Analyses of these components show a significant negative relationship between slow-wave responses to oddball non-targets and perceptual false memory endorsements, suggestive of a link between individual differences in levels of sustained attention and false memory susceptibility. These findings provide the first demonstrated link between individual differences in basic attentional responses and false memory. These results support prior behavioral work linking attention and false memory and highlight the integration between attentional processes and real-world episodic memory.

[1]  A. Kok,et al.  The effect of repetition of infrequent familiar and unfamiliar visual patterns on components of the event-related brain potential , 1980, Biological Psychology.

[2]  John E. Kiat,et al.  Assessing cross-modal target transition effects with a visual-auditory oddball. , 2018, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[3]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection theory: A user's guide, 2nd ed. , 2005 .

[4]  A. Kok On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[5]  Myra A. Fernandes,et al.  Illusory recollection in older adults and younger adults under divided attention. , 2009, Psychology and aging.

[6]  A. Fernández-Bouzas,et al.  Clinical correlations of grey matter reductions in the caudate nucleus of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. , 2010, Journal of psychiatry & neuroscience : JPN.

[7]  B. Lütkenhöner Dipole source localization by means of maximum likelihood estimation. II. Experimental evaluation. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[8]  M. Scherg,et al.  Localizing P300 Generators in Visual Target and Distractor Processing: A Combined Event-Related Potential and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  Jeremy C. Rietschel,et al.  Evidence for a new late positive ERP component in an attended novelty oddball task. , 2010, Psychophysiology.

[10]  Daniel L Schacter,et al.  Not all false memories are created equal: the neural basis of false recognition. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[11]  Jessica A. Grahn,et al.  The role of the basal ganglia in learning and memory: Neuropsychological studies , 2009, Behavioural Brain Research.

[12]  L. Garcia-Larrea,et al.  P3, positive slow wave and working memory load: a study on the functional correlates of slow wave activity. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[13]  P. Derambure,et al.  Role of Basal Ganglia Circuits in Resisting Interference by Distracters: A swLORETA Study , 2012, PloS one.

[14]  E. Golob,et al.  Cortical potentials in an auditory oddball task reflect individual differences in working memory capacity. , 2013, Psychophysiology.

[15]  J. Polich,et al.  Normative Variation of P3a and P3b from a Large Sample , 2007 .

[16]  T W Picton,et al.  Temporal and sequential probability in evoked potential studies. , 1981, Canadian journal of psychology.

[17]  A. Nobre,et al.  Long-term memory prepares neural activity for perception , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[18]  J. Rosenfeld,et al.  Event-related potentials in the dual task paradigm: P300 discriminates engaging and non-engaging films when film-viewing is the primary task. , 1992, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[19]  Sean M. Lane,et al.  Dividing attention during a witnessed event increases eyewitness suggestibility , 2006 .

[20]  W Ritter,et al.  A Review of Event‐Related Potential Components Discovered in the Context of Studying P3 a , 1992, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[21]  Astrid M. Schloerscheidt,et al.  Creating memory illusions: Expectancy-based processing and the generation of false memories , 2002, Memory.

[22]  M. Kane,et al.  Dealing With Prospective Memory Demands While Performing an Ongoing Task: Shared Processing, Increased On-Task Focus, or Both? , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[23]  M. Rivardo,et al.  Integrating Inattentional Blindness and Eyewitness Memory , 2011 .

[24]  Vince D Calhoun,et al.  fMRI in an oddball task: effects of target-to-target interval. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[25]  N. Squires,et al.  Two varieties of long-latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[26]  Sander Nieuwenhuis,et al.  Noradrenergic and cholinergic modulation of late ERP responses to deviant stimuli. , 2015, Psychophysiology.

[27]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Dissociable Controlled Retrieval and Generalized Selection Mechanisms in Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex , 2005, Neuron.

[28]  J. Cohen,et al.  On the number of trials needed for P300. , 1997, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[29]  H L Roediger,et al.  Imagination inflation for action events: Repeated imaginings lead to illusory recollections , 1998, Memory & cognition.

[30]  Bettina Sorger,et al.  Novelty and target processing during an auditory novelty oddball: A simultaneous event-related potential and functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 2008, NeuroImage.

[31]  Michael D. Rugg,et al.  Effects of Divided Attention on fMRI Correlates of Memory Encoding , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  Yuezhi Li,et al.  Localizing P300 generators in high-density event- related potential with fMRI. , 2009, Medical science monitor : international medical journal of experimental and clinical research.

[33]  E. Donchin Presidential address, 1980. Surprise!...Surprise? , 1981, Psychophysiology.

[34]  Qualities of the unreal. , 1986 .

[35]  Godfrey Pearlson,et al.  An adaptive reflexive processing model of neurocognitive function: supporting evidence from a large scale (n = 100) fMRI study of an auditory oddball task , 2005, NeuroImage.

[36]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[37]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[38]  Lorraine Hope,et al.  The effects of divided attention at study and reporting procedure on regulation and monitoring for episodic recall. , 2016, Acta psychologica.

[39]  D. S. Lindsay,et al.  Memory impairment and source misattribution in postevent misinformation experiments with short retention intervals , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[40]  Nicholas B. Turk-Browne,et al.  Memory-guided attention: control from multiple memory systems , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[41]  C. Frith,et al.  Monitoring for target objects: activation of right frontal and parietal cortices with increasing time on task , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  Daniel S. Ruchkin,et al.  11 Positive Slow Wave and P300: Association and Disassociation , 1983 .

[43]  Roberto Hornero,et al.  Auditory P3a and P3b neural generators in schizophrenia: An adaptive sLORETA P300 localization approach , 2015, Schizophrenia Research.

[44]  Matthew A. Palmer,et al.  Phenomenological reports diagnose accuracy of eyewitness identification decisions. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[45]  John Polich,et al.  Affective ERP processing in a visual oddball task: Arousal, valence, and gender , 2008, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[46]  C. Stark,et al.  Neural activity during encoding predicts false memories created by misinformation. , 2005, Learning & memory.

[47]  I. Gotlib,et al.  An attentional scope model of rumination. , 2013, Psychological bulletin.

[48]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .

[49]  F Rösler,et al.  Toward a functional categorization of slow waves: taking into account past and future events. , 1991, Psychophysiology.

[50]  Hongkeun Kim Involvement of the dorsal and ventral attention networks in oddball stimulus processing: A meta‐analysis , 2014, Human brain mapping.

[51]  K. Kiehl,et al.  An event-related fMRI study of visual and auditory oddball tasks , 2001 .

[52]  N. Turk-Browne,et al.  How Hippocampal Memory Shapes, and Is Shaped by, Attention , 2017 .

[53]  H. Blank,et al.  How to protect eyewitness memory against the misinformation effect: A meta-analysis of post-warning studies , 2014 .

[54]  H. Gray,et al.  P300 as an index of attention to self-relevant stimuli , 2004 .

[55]  Robert J. Barry,et al.  Reinstating the Novelty P3 , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[56]  R. Belli,et al.  An exploratory high-density EEG investigation of the misinformation effect: Attentional and recollective differences between true and false perceptual memories , 2017, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[57]  Myra A. Fernandes,et al.  Divided attention and memory: evidence of substantial interference effects at retrieval and encoding. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[58]  Joseph Dien,et al.  The ERP PCA Toolkit: An open source program for advanced statistical analysis of event-related potential data , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[59]  M. L. Kietzman,et al.  Slow wave and P300 in signal detection. , 1980, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[60]  Jocelyn Parong,et al.  The misinformation effect is unrelated to the DRM effect with and without a DRM warning , 2016, Memory.

[61]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data , 2010, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[62]  H. Engeland,et al.  Event-related potentials and performance of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Children and normal controls in auditory and visual selective attention tasks , 1997, Biological Psychiatry.

[63]  C. Gonsalvez,et al.  Can working memory predict target-to-target interval effects in the P300? , 2013, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[64]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .

[65]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. , 2012, Annual review of neuroscience.

[66]  R Näätänen,et al.  Frontal negative and parietal positive components of the slow wave dissociated. , 1987, Psychophysiology.

[67]  B. Lutkenhoner,et al.  Dipole source localization by means of maximum likelihood estimation I. Theory and simulations. , 1998 .

[68]  I Rosén,et al.  Memory for perceived and imagined pictures—an event-related potential study , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[69]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  Large-scale brain networks account for sustained and transient activity during target detection , 2009, NeuroImage.

[70]  D. Rujescu,et al.  The neural basis of the P300 potential , 2004, European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience.

[71]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[72]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Localizing P300 Generators in Visual Target and Distractor Processing: A Combined Event-Related Potential and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[73]  M. Zaragoza,et al.  The Misinformation Effect , 2012 .

[74]  J. Polich Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[75]  E. Loftus Our changeable memories: legal and practical implications , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[76]  Daniel Strüber,et al.  P300 and slow wave from oddball and single-stimulus visual tasks: inter-stimulus interval effects. , 2002, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[77]  A. Coenen,et al.  Neural generators of the auditory evoked potential components P3a and P3b. , 2012, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[78]  Mario F. Mendez,et al.  Neurobehavioral changes associated with caudate lesions , 1989, Neurology.

[79]  Beatrice G. Kuhlmann,et al.  Influences of Source - Item Contingency and Schematic Knowledge on Source Monitoring: Tests of the Probability-Matching Account. , 2011, Journal of memory and language.

[80]  Jessica A. Grahn,et al.  The cognitive functions of the caudate nucleus , 2008, Progress in Neurobiology.

[81]  M. Mesulam,et al.  Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events. , 1999, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[82]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[83]  Christopher Barry,et al.  The effect of divided attention on false memory depends on how memory is tested , 2007, Memory & cognition.

[84]  I. Robertson,et al.  `Oops!': Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[85]  E. Donchin,et al.  Spatiotemporal analysis of the late ERP responses to deviant stimuli. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[86]  Nash Unsworth,et al.  The influence of lapses of attention on working memory capacity , 2016, Memory & cognition.

[87]  S. Lane,et al.  Processing resources and eyewitness suggestibility , 1998 .

[88]  G. Murphy,et al.  Perceptual Load Affects Eyewitness Accuracy and Susceptibility to Leading Questions , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[89]  E F Loftus,et al.  Discrepancy detection and vulnerability to misleading postevent information , 1986, Memory & cognition.

[90]  E. Stein,et al.  Multiple Neuronal Networks Mediate Sustained Attention , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[91]  Michael C. Anderson,et al.  Suppressing Unwanted Memories , 2009 .

[92]  Mara Mather,et al.  (www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/acp.857 Source Monitoring and Suggestibility to Misinformation: Adult Age-Related Differences , 2022 .

[93]  E Donchin,et al.  Second Thoughts : Multiple P 3 OOs Elicited by a Single Stimulus , 2005 .

[94]  A. Engel,et al.  What is novel in the novelty oddball paradigm? Functional significance of the novelty P3 event-related potential as revealed by independent component analysis. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[95]  E. John,et al.  Evoked-Potential Correlates of Stimulus Uncertainty , 1965, Science.

[96]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[97]  R. Benson,et al.  Responses to rare visual target and distractor stimuli using event-related fMRI. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[98]  P. Higham Believing details known to have been suggested , 1998 .

[99]  S. Segalowitz,et al.  Source Monitoring: ERP Evidence for Greater Reactivity to Nontarget Information in Older Adults , 1998, Brain and Cognition.

[100]  Hunter G. Hoffman,et al.  Role of memory strength in reality monitoring decisions: evidence from source attribution biases. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[101]  Vincenzo Varriale,et al.  Impulsivity, intelligence and P300 wave: an empirical study. , 2008, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[102]  M. Brand,et al.  Observation Inflation , 2010, Psychological science.

[103]  P. Sajda,et al.  Simultaneous EEG-fMRI Reveals Temporal Evolution of Coupling between Supramodal Cortical Attention Networks and the Brainstem , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[104]  Vinod Menon,et al.  Where and When the Anterior Cingulate Cortex Modulates Attentional Response: Combined fMRI and ERP Evidence , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[105]  H Pratt,et al.  P300 in response to the subject's own name. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[106]  André Beauducel,et al.  Biopsychological foundations of extraversion differential effort reactivity and state control , 1997 .

[107]  S. Galderisi,et al.  The cortical generators of P3a and P3b: A LORETA study , 2007, Brain Research Bulletin.

[108]  E. Gordon,et al.  THE TEST-RETEST RELIABILITY OF A STANDARDIZED NEUROCOGNITIVE AND NEUROPHYSIOLOGICAL TEST BATTERY: “NEUROMARKER” , 2005, The International journal of neuroscience.

[109]  R. J. Doherty,et al.  Separation of the components of the late positive complex in an ERP dishabituation paradigm , 2005, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[110]  L. McEvoy,et al.  High resolution evoked potential imaging of the cortical dynamics of human working memory. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[111]  Edward F. Jackson,et al.  Caudate Nucleus Volume Asymmetry Predicts Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Symptomatology in Children , 2002, Journal of child neurology.

[112]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[113]  Mateusz Polak,et al.  Toward a Non-memory Misinformation Effect: Accessing the Original Source Does Not Prevent Yielding to Misinformation , 2016 .

[114]  H. Blank,et al.  Effects of Postwarning Specificity on Memory Performance and Confidence in the Eyewitness Misinformation Paradigm , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[115]  M. Chun,et al.  Interactions between attention and memory , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[116]  Joseph Dien,et al.  Evaluating two-step PCA of ERP data with Geomin, Infomax, Oblimin, Promax, and Varimax rotations. , 2010, Psychophysiology.

[117]  P. Fox,et al.  Functional Decoding and Meta-analytic Connectivity Modeling in Adult Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder , 2016, Biological Psychiatry.

[118]  J. Read,et al.  Effects of divided attention and word concreteness on correct recall and false memory reports , 2002, Memory.

[119]  Hong Li,et al.  Temporal features of the degree effect in self-relevance: Neural correlates , 2011, Biological Psychology.

[120]  Role of memory strength in reality monitoring decisions: evidence from source attribution biases. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[121]  Michael C. Anderson,et al.  Suppressing unwanted memories by executive control , 2001, Nature.

[122]  K. McDermott,et al.  Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. , 1995 .