Orientation-specific adaptation to mentally generated lines in human visual cortex

Previous studies have shown that prolonged inspection of a tilted visual pattern leads to changes in perception ("tilt after-effect", TAE), as well as to a reduction of the neural activation evoked by this pattern ("neural adaptation"). In this fMRI study, we investigated whether such perceptual and neural adaptation can be induced solely by mental imagination. The subjects were asked to mentally generate tilted lines, after which they were presented test lines oriented in the same or the direction orthogonal to the mentally tilted lines. Subjects showed a TAE even after mental imagery ("mental TAE"). Furthermore, a significant orientation-specific adaptation occurred in extrastriate visual areas (V3-V4), showing a decreasing gradient of adaptation from areas V4 to V1. Both this neural adaptation and the individual size of the mental TAE correlated with performance in a behavioural task probing mental imagery. Thus, orientation-selective neurons in visual areas seem to be recruited by mental imagery, and the amount of recruitment correlates with the degree of success of mental imagery operations. The influence of mental imagery on perception and on the neural activity in extrastriate visual areas provides evidence for analogue coding of mental images.

[1]  M. Landy,et al.  Orientation-selective adaptation to first- and second-order patterns in human visual cortex. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  S M Kosslyn,et al.  Visual mental imagery interferes with allocentric orientation judgements. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[3]  Lars Muckli,et al.  Primary Visual Cortex Activity along the Apparent-Motion Trace Reflects Illusory Perception , 2005, PLoS biology.

[4]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[5]  J. Gibson,et al.  ADAPTATION , AFTEREFFECT AND CONTRAST IN THE PERCEPTION OF TILTED LINES , 2004 .

[6]  J. Harris,et al.  Contrast, spatial frequency and test duration effects on the tilt aftereffect: Implications for underlying mechanisms , 1989, Vision Research.

[7]  D. F. Marks,et al.  Visual imagery differences in the recall of pictures. , 1973, British journal of psychology.

[8]  G. Boynton,et al.  Orientation-Specific Adaptation in Human Visual Cortex , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  Neural substrates of the tilt illusion. , 1999, Australian and New Zealand journal of ophthalmology.

[10]  J. Tanji,et al.  Role of the lateral prefrontal cortex in executive behavioral control. , 2008, Physiological reviews.

[11]  D. Linden The P300: Where in the Brain Is It Produced and What Does It Tell Us? , 2005, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[12]  D. Tomasi,et al.  Different activation patterns for working memory load and visual attention load , 2007, Brain Research.

[13]  F. Fang,et al.  Duration-dependent FMRI adaptation and distributed viewer-centered face representation in human visual cortex. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[14]  J. Gibson,et al.  Adaptation, after-effect and contrast in the perception of tilted lines. I. Quantitative studies , 1937 .

[15]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  A brief thought can modulate activity in extrastriate visual areas: Top-down effects of refreshing just-seen visual stimuli , 2007, NeuroImage.

[16]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Neural foundations of imagery , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[17]  Leila Montaser-Kouhsari,et al.  Orientation-selective adaptation to illusory contours in human visual cortex. , 2010, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[18]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  What clocks tell us about the neural correlates of spatial imagery , 2004 .

[19]  G. Glover,et al.  Retinotopic organization in human visual cortex and the spatial precision of functional MRI. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.

[20]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Thresholding of Statistical Maps in Functional Neuroimaging Using the False Discovery Rate , 2002, NeuroImage.

[21]  Transfer of tilt after-effects between second-order cues. , 2005, Spatial vision.

[22]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Impact of fMRI Acoustic Noise on the Functional Anatomy of Visual Mental Imagery , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[23]  M. Fabiani,et al.  Changes in brain activity patterns in aging: the novelty oddball. , 1995, Psychophysiology.

[24]  G. Boynton,et al.  Adaptation: from single cells to BOLD signals , 2006, Trends in Neurosciences.

[25]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Mental Imagery of Faces and Places Activates Corresponding Stimulus-Specific Brain Regions , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  A. Dale,et al.  Functional analysis of primary visual cortex (V1) in humans. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[27]  R. Goebel,et al.  Tracking the Mind's Image in the Brain I Time-Resolved fMRI during Visuospatial Mental Imagery , 2002, Neuron.

[28]  Marisa Carrasco,et al.  Feature-based attention modulates orientation-selective responses in human visual cortex , 2010 .

[29]  R. Turner,et al.  Event-Related fMRI: Characterizing Differential Responses , 1998, NeuroImage.

[30]  D. Kersten,et al.  Orientation-tuned FMRI adaptation in human visual cortex. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  P. Montague,et al.  Vividness of mental imagery: Individual variability can be measured objectively , 2007, Vision Research.

[32]  C. Clifford,et al.  A functional angle on some after-effects in cortical vision , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[33]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Brain areas underlying visual mental imagery and visual perception: an fMRI study. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[34]  R. Goebel,et al.  Content- and Task-Specific Dissociations of Frontal Activity during Maintenance and Manipulation in Visual Working Memory , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  J W Belliveau,et al.  Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1995, Science.

[36]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  In search of occipital activation during visual mental imagery , 1994, Trends in Neurosciences.

[37]  S. Kosslyn Image and Brain: The Resolution of the Imagery Debate , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[38]  A. Dale,et al.  Visual motion aftereffect in human cortical area MT revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging , 1995, Nature.

[39]  E. DeYoe,et al.  Mapping striate and extrastriate visual areas in human cerebral cortex. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[40]  W. Singer,et al.  The constructive nature of vision: direct evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of apparent motion and motion imagery , 1998, The European journal of neuroscience.