Orientation-contingent color aftereffects mediated by subjective transparent structures

We examined whether the orientation-contingent color aftereffect (the McCollough effect) could be mediated by subjective horizontal and vertical structure induced by the perception of transparency. In our experiments, red vertical bars and green horizontal bars were alternated as an adapting stimulus. After adaptation, subjects (n=6) were asked to adjust the green and red saturation of a test pattern until they obtained a neutral gray. Horizontal and vertical stripes were combined in the test pattern in three different ways: (1) overlapping with a luminance combination that gave rise to a perception of transparent overlays of horizontal and vertical stripes (valid transparency condition), (2) overlapping with luminance combinations that did not induce a perception of transparency (invalid transparency condition) and that appeased more as a patchwork of checks, and (3) presented in adjacent, nonoverlapping areas. Our results showed that the McCollough effect was significantly greater in the valid transparency condition than in the invalid transparency conditions. The effect in the valid transparency condition was nevertheless less strong than was the effect seen with the standard test stimulus made up of nonoverlapping vertical and horizontal stripes, Our results suggest that the McCollough effect can be mediated by the subjective spatial organization (inner representation of vertical and horizontal stripes) that accompanies the perception of transparency in our stimulus.

[1]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.

[2]  C. McCollough Color Adaptation of Edge-Detectors in the Human Visual System , 1965, Science.

[3]  G. Murch Binocular relationships in a size and color orientation specific aftereffect. , 1972, Journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  P Gouras,et al.  Color and spatial specificity of single units in Rhesus monkey foveal striate cortex. , 1973, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  C. Stromeyer,et al.  Spatial frequency phase effects in human vision. , 1973, Vision research.

[6]  Peter D. Eimas,et al.  Establishment and decay of orientation-contingent aftereffects of color , 1974 .

[7]  F Metelli,et al.  The perception of transparency. , 1974, Scientific American.

[8]  Richard I. Land Monochromatic observations of pattern-induced flicker colors (A) , 1975 .

[9]  D Skowbo,et al.  McCollough effects: experimental findings and theoretical accounts. , 1975, Psychological bulletin.

[10]  D. Mackay,et al.  Dichoptic Induction of Mccollough-Type Effects , 1975, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[11]  R. White New solutions of the refraction integral , 1975 .

[12]  John Uhlarik,et al.  Color aftereffects contingent on perceptual organization , 1977 .

[13]  J. Ross,et al.  McCollough Effect Depends upon Perceived Organization , 1977, Perception.

[14]  C. F. Stromeyer,et al.  Form-Color Aftereffects in Human Vision , 1978 .

[15]  G. Meyer,et al.  Faces, Vases, Subjective Contours, and the McCollough Effect , 1980, Perception.

[16]  Leo Maurice Hurvich,et al.  Color vision , 1981 .

[17]  Richard I. Ivry,et al.  The perception of transparency with achromatic colors , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  DH Hubel,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[19]  W. R. Webster,et al.  Simultaneous color contrast from McCollough effects is spatially contingent , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.

[20]  P. Dodwell,et al.  Global factors generate the mccollough effect , 1987, Vision Research.

[21]  M. Landy,et al.  Transparency and the Cooperative Computation of Scene Attributes , 1991 .