Ecological Knowledge, Leadership, and the Evolution of Menopause in Killer Whales

Classic life-history theory predicts that menopause should not occur because there should be no selection for survival after the cessation of reproduction [1]. Yet, human females routinely live 30 years after they have stopped reproducing [2]. Only two other species-killer whales (Orcinus orca) and short-finned pilot whales (Globicephala macrorhynchus) [3, 4]-have comparable postreproductive lifespans. In theory, menopause can evolve via inclusive fitness benefits [5, 6], but the mechanisms by which postreproductive females help their kin remain enigmatic. One hypothesis is that postreproductive females act as repositories of ecological knowledge and thereby buffer kin against environmental hardships [7, 8]. We provide the first test of this hypothesis using a unique long-term dataset on wild resident killer whales. We show three key results. First, postreproductively aged females lead groups during collective movement in salmon foraging grounds. Second, leadership by postreproductively aged females is especially prominent in difficult years when salmon abundance is low. This finding is critical because salmon abundance drives both mortality and reproductive success in resident killer whales [9, 10]. Third, females are more likely to lead their sons than they are to lead their daughters, supporting predictions of recent models [5] of the evolution of menopause based on kinship dynamics. Our results show that postreproductive females may boost the fitness of kin through the transfer of ecological knowledge. The value gained from the wisdom of elders can help explain why female resident killer whales and humans continue to live long after they have stopped reproducing.

[1]  K. Kramer,et al.  Cooperative breeding and its significance to the demographic success of humans. , 2010 .

[2]  Ilya R. Fischhoff,et al.  Social relationships and reproductive state influence leadership roles in movements of plains zebra, Equus burchellii , 2007, Animal Behaviour.

[3]  Social science: Human reproductive assistance , 2012, Nature.

[4]  Julie Morand-Ferron,et al.  Larger groups of passerines are more efficient problem solvers in the wild , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[5]  K. Hawkes,et al.  Antiquity of postreproductive life: Are there modern impacts on hunter‐gatherer postreproductive life spans? , 2002, American journal of human biology : the official journal of the Human Biology Council.

[6]  T. Vicsek,et al.  Hierarchical group dynamics in pigeon flocks , 2010, Nature.

[7]  Michael A. Thomas,et al.  Complete mitochondrial genome phylogeographic analysis of killer whales (Orcinus orca) indicates multiple species. , 2010, Genome research.

[8]  George C. Williams,et al.  PLEIOTROPY, NATURAL SELECTION, AND THE EVOLUTION OF SENESCENCE , 1957, Science of Aging Knowledge Environment.

[9]  K. Hawkes,et al.  Hadza Women's Time Allocation, Offspring Provisioning, and the Evolution of Long Postmenopausal Life Spans , 1997, Current Anthropology.

[10]  L. Carstensen,et al.  Aging, Emotion, and Evolution , 2003 .

[11]  M. Teaford,et al.  Diet and the evolution of the earliest human ancestors. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[12]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[13]  Peter L. Tyack,et al.  Cetacean societies : field studies of dolphins and whales , 2001 .

[14]  G. Ellis,et al.  Linking killer whale survival and prey abundance: food limitation in the oceans' apex predator? , 2010, Biology Letters.

[15]  J. Krause,et al.  Exploring Animal Social Networks , 2008 .

[16]  Dominic D. P. Johnson,et al.  The Origins and Evolution of Leadership , 2009, Current Biology.

[17]  R. Johnstone,et al.  The evolution of menopause in cetaceans and humans: the role of demography , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[18]  H. Whitehead,et al.  Eusociality, menopause and information in matrilineal whales. , 2005, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[19]  S. Durant,et al.  Matriarchs As Repositories of Social Knowledge in African Elephants , 2001, Science.

[20]  R. Johnstone,et al.  Reproductive conflict and the separation of reproductive generations in humans , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[21]  M. Dahlheim,et al.  Social cohesion among kin, gene flow without dispersal and the evolution of population genetic structure in the killer whale (Orcinus orca) , 2010, Journal of evolutionary biology.

[22]  Charlotte K. Hemelrijk,et al.  Leadership in fish shoals , 2000 .

[23]  M. Dahlheim,et al.  Low genetic variation among killer whales (Orcinus orca) in the eastern north Pacific and genetic differentiation between foraging specialists. , 1998, The Journal of heredity.

[24]  D. Franks,et al.  Adaptive Prolonged Postreproductive Life Span in Killer Whales , 2012, Science.

[25]  J. Kurths,et al.  Chaos–order transition in foraging behavior of ants , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[26]  Daniel W. Franks,et al.  Social networks and models for collective motion in animals , 2011, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[27]  V. Lummaa,et al.  Fitness benefits of prolonged post-reproductive lifespan in women , 2004, Nature.

[28]  I. Couzin,et al.  Effective leadership and decision-making in animal groups on the move , 2005, Nature.

[29]  K. Hawkes,et al.  Grandmothers and the evolution of human longevity: A review of findings and future directions , 2013, Evolutionary anthropology.

[30]  E. Ward,et al.  Quantifying the effects of prey abundance on killer whale reproduction , 2009 .

[31]  E. Charnov,et al.  Grandmothering, menopause, and the evolution of human life histories. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[32]  K. Hyatt,et al.  Status of Anadromous Salmon and Trout in British Columbia and Yukon , 1996 .

[33]  G. Ellis,et al.  Killer whales : the natural history and genealogy of Orinus orca in British Columbia and Washington , 2000 .

[34]  G. Ellis,et al.  Selective foraging by fish-eating killer whales Orcinus orca in British Columbia , 2006 .

[35]  I. Couzin Collective cognition in animal groups , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  L. Carstensen,et al.  Aging, emotion, and evolution: the bigger picture. , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[37]  R. Lackey Pacific Northwest Salmon: Forecasting Their Status in 2100 , 2003 .

[38]  J. S. Peccei A critique of the grandmother hypotheses: Old and new , 2001, American journal of human biology : the official journal of the Human Biology Council.

[39]  J. Poole,et al.  Leadership in elephants: the adaptive value of age , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[40]  Daniel W. Franks,et al.  Social network correlates of food availability in an endangered population of killer whales, Orcinus orca , 2012, Animal Behaviour.

[41]  John K. B. Ford,et al.  Species and stock identification of prey consumed by endangered southern resident killer whales in their summer range , 2010 .