Bridging a Conceptual Divide: How Peer Collaboration Facilitates Science Learning

Collaboration is generally an effective means of learning new information, but is collaboration productive in domains where collaborators may hold qualitatively different conceptions of the domain’s causal structure? We explored this question in the domain of evolutionary biology, where previous research has shown that most individuals construe evolution as the uniform transformation of an entire population (akin to metamorphosis) rather than the selective survival and reproduction of a subset of the population. College undergraduates (n = 44) completed an assessment of their evolutionary reasoning by themselves (pretest), with a partner (dyad test), and several weeks later (posttest). Collaboration proved ineffective for the higherscoring partner in each dyad, as their scores generally remained unchanged from pretest to dyad test to posttest, but it proved effective for the lower-scoring partner. Not only did lowerscoring partners increase their score from pretest to dyad test, but they maintained higher scores at posttest as well. Followup analyses revealed that participants’ posttest scores were predicted by their partners’ pretest scores but only for lowerscoring partners, and the relation was negative: the smaller the difference between pretest score, the greater the gain from pretest to posttest for lower-scoring partners. These findings indicate that collaboration in domains characterized by conceptual change is possible, but that learning from such collaboration is asymmetric (i.e., individuals with low levels of understanding benefit more than their partners do) and unequal (i.e., individuals with low levels of understanding benefit more if their partner’s understanding is only moderately higher). Thus, bridging the gap between a novice’s view of a conceptually complex domain and an expert’s view appears to require instruction more aligned with the former than the latter.

[1]  Stephanie D. Teasley The role of talk in children's peer collaborations. , 1995 .

[2]  G. Walton,et al.  The opportunity to collaborate increases preschoolers' motivation for challenging tasks. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[3]  S. Carey Conceptual Change in Childhood , 1985 .

[4]  Andrew Shtulman,et al.  The Relation Between Essentialist Beliefs and Evolutionary Reasoning , 2008, Cogn. Sci..

[5]  Baruch B. Schwarz,et al.  The Effects of Monological and Dialogical Argumentation on Concept Learning in Evolutionary Theory , 2007 .

[6]  S. Carey The Origin of Concepts , 2000 .

[7]  D. Bates,et al.  Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.

[8]  Michelene T. H. Chi,et al.  Conceptual Change within and across Ontological Categories: Examples from Learning and Discovery in Science , 1992 .

[9]  Patrick J. Leman,et al.  Conceptual Change in Science Is Facilitated Through Peer Collaboration for Boys but Not for Girls. , 2016, Child development.

[10]  Yair Neuman,et al.  Two Wrongs May Make a Right ... If They Argue Together! , 2000 .

[11]  Andrew Shtulman,et al.  Children’s Ability to Learn Evolutionary Explanations for Biological Adaptation , 2016, Young Children’s Developing Understanding of the Biological World.

[12]  Esteban Krotz The Essential tensión: Selected Studies in Scientific Tradition and Change , 1980 .

[13]  Cameron A. J. Ellis,et al.  Conceptions of Evolution Among Science Graduate Students , 2009 .

[14]  Elizabeth A. Gunderson,et al.  Some types of parent number talk count more than others: relations between parents' input and children's cardinal-number knowledge. , 2011, Developmental science.

[15]  Christine Howe,et al.  Collaborative Group Work in Middle Childhood , 2009, Human Development.

[16]  Andrew Shtulman,et al.  Tuition vs. Intuition: Effects of Instruction on Naïve Theories of Evolution , 2013 .

[17]  A. Berti,et al.  Children's Conceptions About the Origin of Species: A Study of Italian Children's Conceptions With and Without Instruction , 2010 .

[18]  H. H. Clark,et al.  References in Conversation Between Experts and Novices , 1987 .

[19]  S. Gelman,et al.  The Essential Child : Origins of Essentialism in Everyday Thought , 2003 .

[20]  Tamer G. Amin,et al.  “Is heat hot?” Inducing conceptual change by integrating everyday and scientific perspectives on thermal phenomena , 2001 .

[21]  Herbert H. Clark,et al.  Contributing to Discourse , 1989, Cogn. Sci..

[22]  Suzanne H. Jones,et al.  Problem-based learning as a facilitator of conceptual change , 2015 .

[23]  Andrew G. Young,et al.  Disagreement and causal learning: others' hypotheses affect children's evaluations of evidence. , 2012, Developmental psychology.

[24]  J. Jipson,et al.  Mother-child conversation and children's understanding of biological and nonbiological changes in size. , 2003, Child development.

[25]  Andrea Gawrylewski Scienceblind: Why Our Intuitive Theories about the World Are So Often Wrong. , 2017 .

[26]  T. Kuhn,et al.  The essential tension : selected studies in scientific tradition and change , 1977 .

[27]  Bryan L. Bonner,et al.  Collective versus individual induction with single versus multiple hypotheses , 1998 .

[28]  When Two Wrongs Make a Right , 1984 .

[29]  Herbert A. Simon,et al.  Collaborative Discovery in a Scientific Domain , 1997, Cogn. Sci..

[30]  Nancy J. Nersessian,et al.  Conceptual change in science and in science education , 1989, Synthese.

[31]  Andrew Shtulman,et al.  Qualitative differences between naïve and scientific theories of evolution , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.

[32]  Charles W. Anderson,et al.  Student conceptions of natural selection and its role in evolution , 1986 .

[33]  S. Vosniadou Capturing and modeling the process of conceptual change. , 1994 .

[34]  John D. Coley,et al.  Relations between Intuitive Biological Thinking and Biological Misconceptions in Biology Majors and Nonmajors , 2015, CBE life sciences education.

[35]  Barbara Rogoff,et al.  Collaborative problem solving and children's planning skills. , 1989 .