Coordination strategies used in stone knapping.

Stone tool-use and manufacture is seen as an important skill during the evolution of our species and recently there has been increased interest in the understanding of perceptual-motor abilities underlying this skill. This study provides further information with respect to the motor strategies used during stone knapping. Kinematics of the striking arm were recorded in expert and novice knappers while producing flakes of two different sizes. Using Uncontrolled Manifold Analysis, the results showed that knappers structure joint angle movements such that the hammer trajectory variability is minimized across trials, with experts displaying significantly smaller variability compared with novices. Principal component analysis further revealed that a single component captures the complexity of the strike and that the strike is governed by movements of the elbow and the wrist. Analysis of movement velocities indicated that both groups adjusted movement velocities according to flake size although experts used smaller hammer, wrist, and elbow velocities in both flake conditions compared with novices. The results suggest that while the gross striking movement is easy to replicate for a novice knapper, it requires prolonged training before a knapper becomes attuned to the finer details necessary for controlled flaking.

[1]  B. Vereijken,et al.  Free(z)ing Degrees of Freedom in Skill Acquisition , 1992 .

[2]  Nicholas Toth,et al.  Behavioral inferences from Early Stone artifact assemblages: an experimental model☆ , 1987 .

[3]  Harold L. Dibble,et al.  Introducing a new experimental design for controlled studies of flake formation: results for exterior platform angle, platform depth, angle of blow, velocity, and force , 2009 .

[4]  Les G. Carlton,et al.  Impulse and Movement Space-Time Variability. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[5]  Aldo A. Faisal,et al.  The Manipulative Complexity of Lower Paleolithic Stone Toolmaking , 2010, PloS one.

[6]  Gary T. Garufi,et al.  Continuing Investigations into the Stone Tool-making and Tool-using Capabilities of a Bonobo (Pan paniscus) , 1999 .

[7]  W. McGrew,et al.  In search of the last common ancestor: new findings on wild chimpanzees , 2010, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[8]  T. Chaminade,et al.  The evolutionary neuroscience of tool making , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  C. A. Putnam,et al.  Sequential motions of body segments in striking and throwing skills: descriptions and explanations. , 1993, Journal of biomechanics.

[10]  F. Raab,et al.  Magnetic Position and Orientation Tracking System , 1979, IEEE Transactions on Aerospace and Electronic Systems.

[11]  Andrew W. Pelcin The Threshold Effect of Platform Width: A Reply to Davis and Shea , 1998 .

[12]  B. Richmond,et al.  Upper limb kinematics and the role of the wrist during stone tool production. , 2010, American journal of physical anthropology.

[13]  G. Schöner,et al.  Analyzing variance in multi-degree-of-freedom movements: uncovering structure versus extracting correlations. , 2007, Motor control.

[14]  Nicholas Toth,et al.  The Oldowan: The Tool Making of Early Hominins and Chimpanzees Compared , 2009 .

[15]  Blandine Bril,et al.  Nesting of asymmetric functions in skilled bimanual action: dynamics of hammering behavior of bead craftsmen. , 2012, Human movement science.

[16]  R Plamondon,et al.  Speed/accuracy trade-offs in target-directed movements , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[17]  Andreas Daffertshofer,et al.  PCA in studying coordination and variability: a tutorial. , 2004, Clinical biomechanics.

[18]  R. Passingham,et al.  Technology, expertise and social cognition in human evolution , 2011, The European journal of neuroscience.

[19]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Hemispheric specialization and functional impact of ipsilesional deficits in movement coordination and accuracy , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  K N An,et al.  Application of a magnetic tracking device to kinesiologic studies. , 1988, Journal of biomechanics.

[21]  G. Dietrich,et al.  Skills and learning difficulties involved in stone knapping: The case of stone‐bead knapping in Khambhat, India , 1995 .

[22]  K. Newell,et al.  Dimensional change in motor learning. , 2001, Human movement science.

[23]  Brian G Richmond,et al.  Hand pressure distribution during Oldowan stone tool production. , 2012, Journal of human evolution.

[24]  Gregor Schöner,et al.  The uncontrolled manifold concept: identifying control variables for a functional task , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  Jeffrey J Flenniken,et al.  The Past, Present, and Future of Flintknapping: An Anthropological Perspective , 1984 .

[26]  Josep Maria Vergès,et al.  What novice knappers have to learn to become expert stone toolmakers , 2010 .

[27]  Satoshi Hirata,et al.  Tool use as a way to assess cognition: how do captive chimpanzees handle the weight of the hammer when cracking a nut? , 2009, Animal Cognition.

[28]  Robert L Sainburg,et al.  Ipsilesional motor deficits following stroke reflect hemispheric specializations for movement control. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[29]  N. Toth The oldowan reassessed: a close look at early stone artifacts , 1985 .

[30]  John D. Speth,et al.  Miscellaneous Studies in Hard-Hammer Percussion Flaking: The Effects of Oblique Impact , 1975, American Antiquity.

[31]  William Andrefsky Lithics: Macroscopic Approaches to Analysis , 1999 .

[32]  Mark L. Latash,et al.  Joint angle variability in 3D bimanual pointing: uncontrolled manifold analysis , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  Richard W. Byrne,et al.  Effects of Manual Disability on Feeding Skills in Gorillas and Chimpanzees , 2002, International Journal of Primatology.

[34]  Kenny R. Coventry,et al.  Does complex behaviour imply complex cognitive abilities , 2002 .

[35]  H. Dibble,et al.  The Effect of Hammer Mass and Velocity on Flake Mass , 1995 .

[36]  Daniel E Lieberman,et al.  Hand biomechanics during simulated stone tool use. , 2011, Journal of human evolution.

[37]  E. Biryukova,et al.  Organization of goal-directed action at a high level of motor skill: the case of stone knapping in India. , 2008, Motor control.

[38]  R. Byrne The Evolution of Thought: The manual skills and cognition that lie behind hominid tool use , 2004 .

[39]  E V Biryukova,et al.  Kinematics of human arm reconstructed from spatial tracking system recordings. , 2000, Journal of biomechanics.

[40]  D. Domkin,et al.  Structure of joint variability in bimanual pointing tasks , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  Carlijn A. Vernooij,et al.  Learning to control orientation and force in a hammering task: The initial stage. , 2012 .

[42]  B. Bril,et al.  How do stone knappers predict and control the outcome of flaking? Implications for understanding early stone tool technology. , 2010, Journal of human evolution.

[43]  R. Sevcik,et al.  Pan the Tool-Maker: Investigations into the Stone Tool-Making and Tool-Using Capabilities of a Bonobo (Pan paniscus) , 1993 .

[44]  F. Multon,et al.  Do handball throws always exhibit a proximal-to-distal segmental sequence? , 2004, Journal of sports sciences.

[45]  Jack Harris,et al.  Primate archaeology , 2009, Nature.

[46]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[47]  B. Elliott,et al.  Long-axis rotation: The missing link in proximal-to-distal segmental sequencing , 2000, Journal of sports sciences.

[48]  M. Marzke,et al.  Hominid hand use in the pliocene and pleistocene: Evidence from experimental archaeology and comparative morphology* , 1986 .

[49]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Lateralization of motor adaptation reveals independence in control of trajectory and steady-state position , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[50]  Naomichi Ogihara,et al.  An analysis of leg joint synergy during bipedal walking in Japanese macaques , 2011, 2011 Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society.

[51]  Gilles Dietrich,et al.  The role of expertise in tool use: skill differences in functional action adaptations to task constraints. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[52]  G. Dietrich,et al.  A Gibsonian Motor Analysis of the Nut-Cracking Technique , 2011 .

[53]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[54]  S. Ambrose Paleolithic Technology and Human Evolution , 2001, Science.

[55]  R. L. Linscheid,et al.  EMG study of hand muscle recruitment during hard hammer percussion manufacture of Oldowan tools. , 1998, American journal of physical anthropology.

[56]  Jeffrey J. Lockman,et al.  What is the role of infant banging in the development of tool use? , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[57]  Mark L. Latash,et al.  The role of kinematic redundancy in adaptation of reaching , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[58]  Karl M. Newell,et al.  Coordination Patterns in Ball Bouncing as a Function of Skill. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[59]  James Steele,et al.  Functional mastery of percussive technology in nut-cracking and stone-flaking actions: experimental comparison and implications for the evolution of the human brain , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.