Coordination strategies used in stone knapping.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] B. Vereijken,et al. Free(z)ing Degrees of Freedom in Skill Acquisition , 1992 .
[2] Nicholas Toth,et al. Behavioral inferences from Early Stone artifact assemblages: an experimental model☆ , 1987 .
[3] Harold L. Dibble,et al. Introducing a new experimental design for controlled studies of flake formation: results for exterior platform angle, platform depth, angle of blow, velocity, and force , 2009 .
[4] Les G. Carlton,et al. Impulse and Movement Space-Time Variability. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.
[5] Aldo A. Faisal,et al. The Manipulative Complexity of Lower Paleolithic Stone Toolmaking , 2010, PloS one.
[6] Gary T. Garufi,et al. Continuing Investigations into the Stone Tool-making and Tool-using Capabilities of a Bonobo (Pan paniscus) , 1999 .
[7] W. McGrew,et al. In search of the last common ancestor: new findings on wild chimpanzees , 2010, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[8] T. Chaminade,et al. The evolutionary neuroscience of tool making , 2007, Neuropsychologia.
[9] C. A. Putnam,et al. Sequential motions of body segments in striking and throwing skills: descriptions and explanations. , 1993, Journal of biomechanics.
[10] F. Raab,et al. Magnetic Position and Orientation Tracking System , 1979, IEEE Transactions on Aerospace and Electronic Systems.
[11] Andrew W. Pelcin. The Threshold Effect of Platform Width: A Reply to Davis and Shea , 1998 .
[12] B. Richmond,et al. Upper limb kinematics and the role of the wrist during stone tool production. , 2010, American journal of physical anthropology.
[13] G. Schöner,et al. Analyzing variance in multi-degree-of-freedom movements: uncovering structure versus extracting correlations. , 2007, Motor control.
[14] Nicholas Toth,et al. The Oldowan: The Tool Making of Early Hominins and Chimpanzees Compared , 2009 .
[15] Blandine Bril,et al. Nesting of asymmetric functions in skilled bimanual action: dynamics of hammering behavior of bead craftsmen. , 2012, Human movement science.
[16] R Plamondon,et al. Speed/accuracy trade-offs in target-directed movements , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[17] Andreas Daffertshofer,et al. PCA in studying coordination and variability: a tutorial. , 2004, Clinical biomechanics.
[18] R. Passingham,et al. Technology, expertise and social cognition in human evolution , 2011, The European journal of neuroscience.
[19] Robert L. Sainburg,et al. Hemispheric specialization and functional impact of ipsilesional deficits in movement coordination and accuracy , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[20] K N An,et al. Application of a magnetic tracking device to kinesiologic studies. , 1988, Journal of biomechanics.
[21] G. Dietrich,et al. Skills and learning difficulties involved in stone knapping: The case of stone‐bead knapping in Khambhat, India , 1995 .
[22] K. Newell,et al. Dimensional change in motor learning. , 2001, Human movement science.
[23] Brian G Richmond,et al. Hand pressure distribution during Oldowan stone tool production. , 2012, Journal of human evolution.
[24] Gregor Schöner,et al. The uncontrolled manifold concept: identifying control variables for a functional task , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[25] Jeffrey J Flenniken,et al. The Past, Present, and Future of Flintknapping: An Anthropological Perspective , 1984 .
[26] Josep Maria Vergès,et al. What novice knappers have to learn to become expert stone toolmakers , 2010 .
[27] Satoshi Hirata,et al. Tool use as a way to assess cognition: how do captive chimpanzees handle the weight of the hammer when cracking a nut? , 2009, Animal Cognition.
[28] Robert L Sainburg,et al. Ipsilesional motor deficits following stroke reflect hemispheric specializations for movement control. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[29] N. Toth. The oldowan reassessed: a close look at early stone artifacts , 1985 .
[30] John D. Speth,et al. Miscellaneous Studies in Hard-Hammer Percussion Flaking: The Effects of Oblique Impact , 1975, American Antiquity.
[31] William Andrefsky. Lithics: Macroscopic Approaches to Analysis , 1999 .
[32] Mark L. Latash,et al. Joint angle variability in 3D bimanual pointing: uncontrolled manifold analysis , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[33] Richard W. Byrne,et al. Effects of Manual Disability on Feeding Skills in Gorillas and Chimpanzees , 2002, International Journal of Primatology.
[34] Kenny R. Coventry,et al. Does complex behaviour imply complex cognitive abilities , 2002 .
[35] H. Dibble,et al. The Effect of Hammer Mass and Velocity on Flake Mass , 1995 .
[36] Daniel E Lieberman,et al. Hand biomechanics during simulated stone tool use. , 2011, Journal of human evolution.
[37] E. Biryukova,et al. Organization of goal-directed action at a high level of motor skill: the case of stone knapping in India. , 2008, Motor control.
[38] R. Byrne. The Evolution of Thought: The manual skills and cognition that lie behind hominid tool use , 2004 .
[39] E V Biryukova,et al. Kinematics of human arm reconstructed from spatial tracking system recordings. , 2000, Journal of biomechanics.
[40] D. Domkin,et al. Structure of joint variability in bimanual pointing tasks , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[41] Carlijn A. Vernooij,et al. Learning to control orientation and force in a hammering task: The initial stage. , 2012 .
[42] B. Bril,et al. How do stone knappers predict and control the outcome of flaking? Implications for understanding early stone tool technology. , 2010, Journal of human evolution.
[43] R. Sevcik,et al. Pan the Tool-Maker: Investigations into the Stone Tool-Making and Tool-Using Capabilities of a Bonobo (Pan paniscus) , 1993 .
[44] F. Multon,et al. Do handball throws always exhibit a proximal-to-distal segmental sequence? , 2004, Journal of sports sciences.
[45] Jack Harris,et al. Primate archaeology , 2009, Nature.
[46] P. Fitts. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.
[47] B. Elliott,et al. Long-axis rotation: The missing link in proximal-to-distal segmental sequencing , 2000, Journal of sports sciences.
[48] M. Marzke,et al. Hominid hand use in the pliocene and pleistocene: Evidence from experimental archaeology and comparative morphology* , 1986 .
[49] Robert L. Sainburg,et al. Lateralization of motor adaptation reveals independence in control of trajectory and steady-state position , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[50] Naomichi Ogihara,et al. An analysis of leg joint synergy during bipedal walking in Japanese macaques , 2011, 2011 Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society.
[51] Gilles Dietrich,et al. The role of expertise in tool use: skill differences in functional action adaptations to task constraints. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[52] G. Dietrich,et al. A Gibsonian Motor Analysis of the Nut-Cracking Technique , 2011 .
[53] Michael I. Jordan,et al. Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[54] S. Ambrose. Paleolithic Technology and Human Evolution , 2001, Science.
[55] R. L. Linscheid,et al. EMG study of hand muscle recruitment during hard hammer percussion manufacture of Oldowan tools. , 1998, American journal of physical anthropology.
[56] Jeffrey J. Lockman,et al. What is the role of infant banging in the development of tool use? , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.
[57] Mark L. Latash,et al. The role of kinematic redundancy in adaptation of reaching , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[58] Karl M. Newell,et al. Coordination Patterns in Ball Bouncing as a Function of Skill. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.
[59] James Steele,et al. Functional mastery of percussive technology in nut-cracking and stone-flaking actions: experimental comparison and implications for the evolution of the human brain , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.