A Sampling Model of Social Judgment
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J. Kruschke,et al. ALCOVE: an exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning. , 1992, Psychological review.
[2] L. Festinger. A Theory of Social Comparison Processes , 1954 .
[3] James J. Gross,et al. Misery Has More Company Than People Think: Underestimating the Prevalence of Others’ Negative Emotions , 2011, Personality & social psychology bulletin.
[4] Eliot R. Smith,et al. Exemplar-Based Model of Social Judgment , 1992 .
[5] Henrik Olsson,et al. Social Sampling Explains Apparent Biases in Judgments of Social Environments , 2012, Psychological science.
[6] J. Krueger,et al. Estimates of Social Consensus by Majorities and Minorities: The Case for Social Projection , 1997, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.
[7] Norman Miller,et al. Ten years of research on the false-consensus effect: an empirical and theoretical review , 1987 .
[8] R. Huckfeldt,et al. Citizens, Politics and Social Communication: Information and Influence in an Election Campaign , 1995 .
[9] Olesya Govorun,et al. Better-than-Average Effect , 2020, Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences.
[10] J. Townsend,et al. The serial-parallel dilemma: A case study in a linkage of theory and method , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[11] Martin A. Nowak,et al. The Evolution of Homophily , 2012, Scientific Reports.
[12] Robbie M. Sutton,et al. Why Wealthier People Think People Are Wealthier, and Why It Matters , 2015, Psychological science.
[13] J. Jonides,et al. Facebook Use Predicts Declines in Subjective Well-Being in Young Adults , 2013, PloS one.
[14] A. Parducci. Category judgment: a range-frequency model. , 1965, Psychological review.
[15] M. Hilbert,et al. Toward a synthesis of cognitive biases: how noisy information processing can bias human decision making. , 2012, Psychological bulletin.
[16] R. Nosofsky,et al. An exemplar-based random walk model of speeded classification. , 1997, Psychological review.
[17] K. Fiedler,et al. On splitting and merging categories: A regression account of subadditivity , 2009, Memory & cognition.
[18] Ulrich Hoffrage,et al. Simple heuristics in a social world , 2012 .
[19] J. Krueger,et al. Self-enhancement diminished. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[20] Bella Struminskaya,et al. Survey Data Documentation , 2018 .
[21] Joseph B. Bayer,et al. Passive Facebook usage undermines affective well-being: Experimental and longitudinal evidence. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[22] Brian Mullen,et al. In-group-out-group differences in social projection , 1992 .
[23] J. Krueger. From social projection to social behaviour , 2007 .
[24] Richard Coles,et al. Summary health statistics for U.S. adults: national health interview survey, 2012. , 2014, Vital and health statistics. Series 10, Data from the National Health Survey.
[25] J. Krueger,et al. The truly false consensus effect: an ineradicable and egocentric bias in social perception. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[26] A. Wood,et al. Social norm influences on evaluations of the risks associated with alcohol consumption: applying the rank-based decision by sampling model to health judgments. , 2012, Alcohol and alcoholism.
[27] Junqi Shi,et al. Economic Inequality Is Linked to Biased Self-Perception , 2011, Psychological science.
[28] L. Rips,et al. The Psychology of Survey Response , 2000 .
[29] P. Salovey,et al. Perceived distributions of the characteristics of in-group and out-group members: empirical evidence and a computer simulation. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[30] R. Nosofsky. Attention, similarity, and the identification-categorization relationship. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[31] N. Christakis,et al. SUPPLEMENTARY ONLINE MATERIAL FOR: The Collective Dynamics of Smoking in a Large Social Network , 2022 .
[32] Kerri L. Johnson,et al. Why the Unskilled are Unaware: Further Explorations of (Absent) Self-Insight Among the Incompetent , 2006, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.
[33] J. Murabito,et al. The Spread of Alcohol Consumption Behavior in a Large Social Network , 2010, Annals of Internal Medicine.
[34] F. Kreuter. Improving Surveys with Paradata , 2013 .
[35] M. McPherson,et al. Birds of a Feather: Homophily in Social Networks , 2001 .
[36] Robyn M. Dawes,et al. The False Consensus Effect and Overconfidence: Flaws in Judgment or Flaws in How We Study Judgment? , 1996 .
[37] R. Nisbett,et al. Perception of social distributions. , 1985, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[38] N. Christakis,et al. The Spread of Obesity in a Large Social Network Over 32 Years , 2007, The New England journal of medicine.
[39] Richard P. Larrick,et al. Skilled or unskilled, but still unaware of it: how perceptions of difficulty drive miscalibration in relative comparisons. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[40] Eliot R. Smith,et al. Agent-Based Modeling: A New Approach for Theory Building in Social Psychology , 2007, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.
[41] M. L. Klotz,et al. Personal contact, individuation, and the better-than-average effect. , 1995 .
[42] Deborah A Small,et al. Error and bias in comparative judgment: on being both better and worse than we think we are. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[43] Henrik Olsson,et al. Naive empiricism and dogmatism in confidence research: a critical examination of the hard-easy effect. , 2000 .
[44] James M. Fields,et al. Public Beliefs About the Beliefs of the Public , 1976 .
[45] P. Juslin,et al. Naive empiricism and dogmatism in confidence research: a critical examination of the hard-easy effect. , 2000, Psychological review.
[46] Shelley E. Taylor,et al. Theory and Research Concerning Social Comparisons of Personal Attributes , 2001 .
[47] C. Lebiere,et al. The Atomic Components of Thought , 1998 .
[48] D. Moore,et al. The trouble with overconfidence. , 2008, Psychological review.
[49] Ulrike Hahn,et al. Unrealistic optimism about future life events: a cautionary note. , 2011, Psychological review.
[50] R. Dawes. Statistical criteria for establishing a truly false consensus effect , 1989 .
[51] Ruth S Ogden,et al. Do Student Evaluations of University Reflect Inaccurate Beliefs or Actual Experience? A Relative Rank Model , 2014, Journal of behavioral decision making.
[52] Y. Klar,et al. No one in my group can be below the group's average: a robust positivity bias in favor of anonymous peers. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[53] Richard M. Shiffrin,et al. Familiarity and categorization processes in memory search , 2014, Cognitive Psychology.
[54] M. Alicke. Global self-evaluation as determined by the desirability and controllability of trait adjectives. , 1985 .
[55] Jonathon D. Brown,et al. Understanding the Better Than Average Effect , 2012, Personality & social psychology bulletin.
[56] R. Nosofsky. Attention, similarity, and the identification-categorization relationship. , 1986 .
[57] Joachim I. Krueger,et al. The projective perception of the social world: A building block of social comparison processes. , 2000 .
[58] Andreas Graefe,et al. Accuracy of Vote Expectation Surveys in Forecasting Elections , 2014 .
[59] D. Funder,et al. Towards a balanced social psychology: Causes, consequences, and cures for the problem-seeking approach to social behavior and cognition , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[60] J. Krueger,et al. Social Projection to Ingroups and Outgroups: A Review and Meta-Analysis , 2005, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.
[61] V. Signorile,et al. A Test of Significance for the Homophily Index , 1965, American Journal of Sociology.
[62] James M. Olson,et al. Better, Stronger, Faster: Self-Serving Judgment, Affect Regulation, and the Optimal Vigilance Hypothesis , 2007, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.
[63] David Dunning,et al. Evidence for egocentric comparison in social judgment , 1996 .
[64] J. Krueger,et al. Comparisons in research and reasoning: Toward an integrative theory of social induction , 2013 .
[65] Walter Quattrociocchi,et al. Echo Chambers on Facebook , 2016 .
[66] Betsy Sinclair,et al. The Social Citizen: Peer Networks and Political Behavior , 2012 .
[67] D. E. Frable,et al. Being and feeling unique: statistical deviance and psychological marginality. , 1993, Journal of personality.
[68] I. Erev,et al. Simultaneous Over- and Underconfidence: The Role of Error in Judgment Processes. , 1994 .
[69] Klaus Fiedler,et al. Information Sampling and Adaptive Cognition , 2005 .
[70] D. Moore. When good = better than average , 2007, Judgment and Decision Making.
[71] Gordon D. A. Brown,et al. Decision by sampling , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.
[72] K. Fiedler. Explaining and simulating judgment biases as an aggregation phenomenon in probabilistic, multiple-cue environments. , 1996 .
[73] C. Sedikides,et al. Pancultural Self-Enhancement , 2002 .
[74] P. Ditto,et al. Motivated Happiness , 2014 .
[75] C. Judd,et al. Accuracy in the judgment of in-group and out-group variability. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[76] J. Suls,et al. In search of the false-uniqueness phenomenon: fear and estimates of social consensus. , 1987, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[77] Eliot R. Smith,et al. Contextualizing person perception: distributed social cognition. , 2009, Psychological review.
[78] Neil D. Weinstein,et al. Egocentrism as a Source of Unrealistic Optimism , 1982 .
[79] John R. Anderson,et al. The Adaptive Character of Thought , 1990 .
[80] P. Thagard,et al. FORMING IMPRESSIONS FROM STEREOTYPES, TRAITS, AND BEHAVIORS : A PARALLEL-CONSTRAINT-SATISFACTION THEORY , 1996 .
[81] Anja Eller,et al. Improving intergroup relations through direct, extended and other forms of indirect contact , 2011 .
[82] K. Fiedler. Beware of samples! A cognitive-ecological sampling approach to judgment biases. , 2000, Psychological review.
[83] A. Kapteyn,et al. Asking about social circles improves election predictions , 2018, Nature Human Behaviour.
[84] Thorsten Pachur,et al. Intuitive judgments of social statistics : How exhaustive does sampling need to be? , 2013 .
[85] John R. Chambers,et al. Biases in social comparative judgments: the role of nonmotivated factors in above-average and comparative-optimism effects. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.
[86] Magnus Persson,et al. PROBabilities from EXemplars (PROBEX): a "lazy" algorithm for probabilistic inference from generic knowledge , 2002 .
[87] Paul K. J. Han,et al. Presenting quantitative information about decision outcomes: a risk communication primer for patient decision aid developers , 2013, BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making.
[88] Tai Gyu Kim,et al. Myopic social prediction and the solo comparison effect. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[89] N. Epley,et al. Perspective taking as egocentric anchoring and adjustment. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[90] H. Simon,et al. The sciences of the artificial (3rd ed.) , 1996 .
[91] P. Todd,et al. Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart , 1999 .
[92] Pancultural self-enhancement. , 2003 .
[93] J. Kruger,et al. Unskilled and unaware of it: how difficulties in recognizing one's own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[94] J. Kruger. Lake Wobegon be gone! The "below-average effect" and the egocentric nature of comparative ability judgments. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[95] Thomas T. Hills,et al. Dynamic search and working memory in social recall. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[96] J. Coleman. Relational Analysis: The Study of Social Organizations with Survey Methods , 1958 .
[97] J. Krueger. On the Perception of Social Consensus , 1998 .
[98] Anders Winman,et al. The naïve intuitive statistician: a naïve sampling model of intuitive confidence intervals. , 2007, Psychological review.
[99] David Dunning,et al. The Mixed Blessings of Self-Knowledge in Behavioral Prediction: Enhanced Discrimination but Exacerbated Bias , 2006, Personality & social psychology bulletin.
[100] Jerry Suls,et al. False Consensus and False Uniqueness in Estimating the Prevalence of Health‐Protective Behaviors , 1988 .
[101] P. Juslin,et al. Format dependence in subjective probability calibration , 1999 .
[102] R. H. Smith,et al. Intrapersonal and social comparison determinants of happiness: a range-frequency analysis. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[103] Allen Parducci,et al. Happiness, Pleasure, and Judgment: The Contextual Theory and Its Applications , 1995 .
[104] Brian Mullen,et al. The false consensus effect: A meta-analysis of 115 hypothesis tests , 1985 .
[105] N. Weinstein. Unrealistic optimism about future life events , 1980 .
[106] Roger Ratcliff,et al. Pointing, looking at, and pressing keys: A diffusion model account of response modality. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[107] Joachim Krueger,et al. Unskilled, unaware, or both? The better-than-average heuristic and statistical regression predict errors in estimates of own performance. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[108] L. Ross,et al. The “false consensus effect”: An egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes , 1977 .
[109] Michael J. Liersch,et al. People Believe That They Are Prototypically Good or Bad. , 2013, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.
[110] Henrik Olsson,et al. False Consensus About False Consensus , 2013, CogSci.
[111] Dirk Van Rooy,et al. A recurrent connectionist model of group biases. , 2003, Psychological review.
[112] R. White. Get the data. , 2001, JEMS : a journal of emergency medical services.