Hands in the air: using ungrounded iconic gestures to teach children conservation of quantity.

Including gesture in instruction facilitates learning. Why? One possibility is that gesture points out objects in the immediate context and thus helps ground the words learners hear in the world they see. Previous work on gesture's role in instruction has used gestures that either point to or trace paths on objects, thus providing support for this hypothesis. The experiments described here investigated the possibility that gesture helps children learn even when it is not produced in relation to an object but is instead produced "in the air." Children were given instruction in Piagetian conservation problems with or without gesture and with or without concrete objects. The results indicate that children given instruction with speech and gesture learned more about conservation than children given instruction with speech alone, whether or not objects were present during instruction. Gesture in instruction can thus help learners learn even when those gestures do not direct attention to visible objects, suggesting that gesture can do more for learners than simply ground arbitrary, symbolic language in the physical, observable world.

[1]  S. Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Gesturing makes learning last , 2008, Cognition.

[2]  Michael J. Spivey,et al.  Oculomotor mechanisms activated by imagery and memory: eye movements to absent objects , 2001, Psychological research.

[3]  P. Lachenbruch Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.) , 1989 .

[4]  M. Studdert-Kennedy Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal About Thought. , 1994 .

[5]  Robert L. Goldstone,et al.  The transfer of abstract principles governing complex adaptive systems , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.

[6]  D. Gentner,et al.  Comparison in the Development of Categories , 1999 .

[7]  S. Goldin-Meadow,et al.  The mismatch between gesture and speech as an index of transitional knowledge , 1986, Cognition.

[8]  R Sun,et al.  Proceedings of the 28th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society , 2006 .

[9]  K. Holyoak,et al.  Cognitive Supports for Analogies in the Mathematics Classroom , 2007, Science.

[10]  S. Kelly,et al.  Broadening the units of analysis in communication: speech and nonverbal behaviours in pragmatic comprehension , 2001, Journal of Child Language.

[11]  Richard Gwyn,et al.  The Semiotic Body in its Environment , 2001 .

[12]  S Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Comprehension and production of gesture in combination with speech in one-word speakers , 1992, Journal of Child Language.

[13]  S. Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Hearing Gesture: How Our Hands Help Us Think , 2003 .

[14]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[15]  D. Gentner,et al.  Making a silk purse out of two sow's ears: young children's use of comparison in category learning. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[16]  S. Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Making children gesture brings out implicit knowledge and leads to learning. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[17]  Shahrzad Mahootian,et al.  The Role of Gesture in Bilingual Education: Does Gesture Enhance Learning? , 2004 .

[18]  Vladimir M. Sloutsky,et al.  Do Children Need Concrete Instantiations to Learn an Abstract Concept , 2006 .

[19]  S. Goldin-Meadow,et al.  The Role of Gesture in Learning: Do Children Use Their Hands to Change Their Minds? , 2006 .

[20]  Arthur M. Glenberg,et al.  Indexical understanding of instructions , 1999 .

[21]  E. Goldfield,et al.  The use and effectiveness of maternal scaffolding in mother-infant games. , 1984, Child development.

[22]  M. Alibali,et al.  Gesture-Speech Mismatch and Mechanisms of Learning: What the Hands Reveal about a Child′s State of Mind , 1993, Cognitive Psychology.

[23]  Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al.  From children's hands to adults' ears: gesture's role in the learning process. , 2003, Developmental psychology.

[24]  Susan M. Wagner,et al.  Probing the Mental Representation of Gesture: Is Handwaving Spatial?. , 2004 .

[25]  Martha W. Alibali,et al.  The Role of Gesture in Children's Comprehension of Spoken Language:Now They Need It, Now They Don't , 2000 .

[26]  Spencer D. Kelly,et al.  Can Children Detect Conceptual Information Conveyed Through Other Children's Nonverbal Behaviors? , 1997 .

[27]  Michelle Perry,et al.  How many do you see? The use of nonspoken representations in first-grade mathematics lessons , 2001 .

[28]  M. Alibali,et al.  Transitions in concept acquisition: using the hand to read the mind. , 1993, Psychological review.

[29]  Susan M. Wagner,et al.  Explaining Math: Gesturing Lightens the Load , 2001, Psychological science.

[30]  Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Transitional knowledge in the acquisition of concepts , 1988 .

[31]  Wolff‐Michael Roth,et al.  From activity to gestures and scientific language , 2001 .

[32]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Teachers’ gestures facilitate students’ learning: A lesson in symmetry , 2003 .

[33]  Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Children Learn When Their Teacher's Gestures and Speech Differ , 2005, Psychological science.

[34]  K. Pine,et al.  More gestures than answers: children learning about balance. , 2004, Developmental psychology.