Number of perceptually distinct surface colors in natural scenes.

The ability to perceptually identify distinct surfaces in natural scenes by virtue of their color depends not only on the relative frequency of surface colors but also on the probabilistic nature of observer judgments. Previous methods of estimating the number of discriminable surface colors, whether based on theoretical color gamuts or recorded from real scenes, have taken a deterministic approach. Thus, a three-dimensional representation of the gamut of colors is divided into elementary cells or points which are spaced at one discrimination-threshold unit intervals and which are then counted. In this study, information-theoretic methods were used to take into account both differing surface-color frequencies and observer response uncertainty. Spectral radiances were calculated from 50 hyperspectral images of natural scenes and were represented in a perceptually almost uniform color space. The average number of perceptually distinct surface colors was estimated as 7.3 × 10(3), much smaller than that based on counting methods. This number is also much smaller than the number of distinct points in a scene that are, in principle, available for reliable identification under illuminant changes, suggesting that color constancy, or the lack of it, does not generally determine the limit on the use of color for surface identification.

[1]  D. C. Rich,et al.  Publication CIE 159: A colour appearance model for colour management systems: CIECAM02 , 2006 .

[2]  R. A. Weale,et al.  Color in Business, Science and Industry, 3rd ed , 1976 .

[3]  S. Hecht,et al.  The colors of natural objects and terrains, and their relation to visual color deficiency. , 1949, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[4]  M. R. Pointer,et al.  On the number of discernible colours , 1998 .

[5]  D. L. Macadam The Theory of the Maximum Visual Efficiency of Colored Materials , 1935 .

[6]  M. Luo,et al.  Uniform colour spaces based on CIECAM02 colour appearance model , 2006 .

[7]  L. T. DeCarlo Signal detection theory and generalized linear models , 1998 .

[8]  Francisco Martínez-Verdú,et al.  Computation and visualization of the MacAdam limits for any lightness, hue angle, and light source. , 2007, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[9]  Claude E. Shannon,et al.  The mathematical theory of communication , 1950 .

[10]  M. N. Goria,et al.  A new class of random vector entropy estimators and its applications in testing statistical hypotheses , 2005 .

[11]  Dorothy Nickerson,et al.  A Psychological Color Solid , 1943 .

[12]  D. Foster,et al.  Frequency of metamerism in natural scenes , 2006 .

[13]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection theory: A user's guide, 2nd ed. , 2005 .

[14]  Ron Gershon,et al.  Measurement and Analysis of Object Reflectance Spectra , 1994 .

[15]  Lawrence T. DeCarlo,et al.  Signal detection theory and generalized linear models , 1998 .

[16]  C. D. Kemp,et al.  Density Estimation for Statistics and Data Analysis , 1987 .

[17]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .

[18]  Thomas M. Cover,et al.  Elements of Information Theory , 2005 .

[19]  M. Luo,et al.  The development of the CIE 2000 Colour Difference Formula , 2001 .

[20]  M. Webster,et al.  Adaptation and the color statistics of natural images , 1997, Vision Research.

[21]  Kinjiro Amano,et al.  Approaching ideal observer efficiency in using color to retrieve information from natural scenes. , 2009, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[22]  Rafael Huertas,et al.  Performance of recent advanced color-difference formulas using the standardized residual sum of squares index. , 2008, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[23]  D. Foster,et al.  Frequency of metamerism in natural scenes. , 2007, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[24]  T. R. J. Bossomaier,et al.  Human cone-pigment spectral sensitivities and the reflectances of natural surfaces , 1992, Biological Cybernetics.

[25]  G. J. Burton,et al.  Color and spatial structure in natural scenes. , 1987, Applied optics.

[26]  Flávio P. Ferreira,et al.  Statistics of spatial cone-excitation ratios in natural scenes. , 2002, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[27]  J. Parkkinen,et al.  Classification of the reflectance spectra of pine, spruce, and birch. , 1994, Applied optics.

[28]  David H Brainard,et al.  Color and material perception: achievements and challenges. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[29]  Donald I. A. MacLeod,et al.  The pleistochrome: optimal opponent codes for natural colours , 2003 .

[30]  Brian A. Wandell,et al.  A spatial extension of CIELAB for digital color‐image reproduction , 1997 .

[31]  S. Nascimento,et al.  The number of discernible colors in natural scenes. , 2008, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[32]  A. Kraskov,et al.  Estimating mutual information. , 2003, Physical review. E, Statistical, nonlinear, and soft matter physics.

[33]  Sang Joon Kim,et al.  A Mathematical Theory of Communication , 2006 .

[34]  Guihua Cui,et al.  Uniform colour spaces based on the DIN99 colour‐difference formula , 2002 .

[35]  M. Pointer The Gamut of Real Surface Colours , 1980 .

[36]  Mark D. Fairchild,et al.  iCAM framework for image appearance, differences, and quality , 2004, J. Electronic Imaging.

[37]  C. McCamy,et al.  On the number of discernible colors , 1998 .

[38]  D. B. Judd,et al.  Color in Business, Science, and Industry , 1953 .