Kinesthetic Sensing

The term kinesthesia refers to the perception of limb movement and position, and is often broadly defined to include the perception of force as well. These sensory perceptions originate primarily from the activity of mechanoreceptors in muscles, which provides the central nervous system with information about the static length of muscles, the rate at which muscle length changes, and the forces muscles generate. From these signals comes our awareness of where our limbs are in space, when our limbs have moved, and the mechanical properties of objects (e.g. weight, compliance) with which they interact. Sensory information about changes in limb position and movement also arises from other sources, namely receptors in the skin and joints. These inputs appear to be particularly important for kinesthesia in the hand, as both joint (Clark et al., 1989; Ferrell et al., 1987) and (or) cutaneous anesthesia (Clark et al., 1986) impairs the ability to detect finger movements and perceive finger positions. For more proximal joints, such as the knee, joint and (or) skin anesthesia does not have a significant influence on the perception of limb position (Clark et al., 1979). It appears that for the hand, cutaneous receptors provide an important facilitatory input to the central nervous system that is used to interpret position and movement signals arising from other sources. Cutaneous receptors in the hairy skin on the dorsum of the hand are capable, however, of encoding joint movement very precisely via their responses to stretch of the skin overlying the active joint (Collins & Prochazka, 1996; Edin, 1992). The importance of cutaneous sensory feedback to the perception of finger movements and positions is not surprising in view of the high innervation density of cutaneous mechanoreceptors in the hand, and its specialization for tactile exploration and manipulation. This feedback may also be more important for kinesthesia in the hand than for other parts of the body because of the complex anatomical arrangement of muscles, with most muscles acting over several finger joints, which would result in a considerable ambiguity of muscle spindle receptor discharges. In addition to these peripherally originating signals, there is evidence that central (cortical) feedback pathways provide information that is used to decode muscle afferent signals and in the perception of force.

[1]  J. Paillard,et al.  Active and passive movements in the calibration of position sense , 1968 .

[2]  F. J. Clark,et al.  Contributions of cutaneous and joint receptors to static knee-position sense in man. , 1979, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  F Buchthal,et al.  Motor unit of mammalian muscle. , 1980, Physiological reviews.

[4]  M. S. Devanandan,et al.  A quantitative study of muscle spindles and tendon organs in some intrinsic muscles of the hand in the bonnet monkey (Macaca radiata) , 1983, The Anatomical record.

[5]  I. W. Hunter,et al.  Effect of fatigue on force sensation , 1983, Experimental Neurology.

[6]  D. McCloskey,et al.  Detections of movements imposed on finger, elbow and shoulder joints. , 1983, The Journal of physiology.

[7]  M. Hulliger,et al.  The mammalian muscle spindle and its central control. , 1984, Reviews of physiology, biochemistry and pharmacology.

[8]  F. J. Clark,et al.  Role of intramuscular receptors in the awareness of limb position. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  F. J. Clark,et al.  Proprioception with the proximal interphalangeal joint of the index finger. Evidence for a movement sense without a static-position sense. , 1986, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[10]  D. McCloskey,et al.  The role of joint receptors in human kinaesthesia when intramuscular receptors cannot contribute. , 1987, The Journal of physiology.

[11]  F. J. Clark,et al.  The contribution of articular receptors to proprioception with the fingers in humans. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  R. J. Irwin,et al.  Differential thresholds for limb movement measured using adaptive techniques , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[13]  L. Jami Golgi tendon organs in mammalian skeletal muscle: functional properties and central actions. , 1992, Physiological reviews.

[14]  B. Edin Quantitative analysis of static strain sensitivity in human mechanoreceptors from hairy skin. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Hong Z. Tan,et al.  HUMAN FACTORS FOR THE DESIGN OF FORCE-REFLECTING HAPTIC INTERFACES , 1994 .

[16]  Mandayam A. Srinivasan,et al.  Manual resolution of viscosity and mass , 1995 .

[17]  M A Srinivasan,et al.  Manual discrimination of compliance using active pinch grasp: The roles of force and work cues , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  L. Rowell,et al.  Exercise : regulation and integration of multiple systems , 1996 .

[19]  D. F. Collins,et al.  Movement illusions evoked by ensemble cutaneous input from the dorsum of the human hand. , 1996, The Journal of physiology.

[20]  I Hunter,et al.  Viscosity Discrimination: A Comparison of an Adaptive Two-Alternative Forced-Choice and an Adjustment Procedure , 1997, Perception.

[21]  J. Hermsdörfer,et al.  Changes in perceived finger force produced by muscular contractions under isometric and anisometric conditions , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  I. Cathers,et al.  Tracking performance with sinusoidal and irregular targets under different conditions of peripheral feedback , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  H. Freund,et al.  Invariant temporal characteristics of manipulative hand movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  I. W. Hunter,et al.  Influence of the mechanical properties of a manipulandum on human operator dynamics , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.