Task-irrelevant angry faces capture attention in visual search while modulated by resources.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J. Theeuwes,et al. Abrupt onsets capture attention independent of top-down control settings , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.
[2] Gilles Pourtois,et al. Neural systems for orienting attention to the location of threat signals: An event-related fMRI study , 2006, NeuroImage.
[3] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[4] Jan Theeuwes,et al. The limits of top-down control of visual attention. , 2009, Acta psychologica.
[5] Joseph E. LeDoux,et al. How danger is encoded: towards a systems, cellular, and computational understanding of cognitive-emotional interactions , 1999 .
[6] Odmar Neumann,et al. Automatic Processing: A Review of Recent Findings and a Plea for an Old Theory , 1984 .
[7] J. Theeuwes. Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[8] E. Fox,et al. Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[9] Caught by the evil eye : nonconscious information processing, emotion, and attention to facial stimuli , 2005 .
[10] E. Fox,et al. Attentional bias for threat: Evidence for delayed disengagement from emotional faces , 2002, Cognition & emotion.
[11] G. Crombez,et al. Attention for emotional faces under restricted awareness revisited: do emotional faces automatically attract attention? , 2007, Emotion.
[12] R. Dolan,et al. Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala , 1998, Nature.
[13] A. Öhman,et al. Automatically elicited fear: Conditioned skin conductance responses to masked facial expressions , 1994 .
[14] L. Pessoa,et al. Load-dependent modulation of affective picture processing , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[15] G. Alpers,et al. Here is looking at you: emotional faces predominate in binocular rivalry. , 2007, Emotion.
[16] H. Okon-Singer,et al. Distinguishing between automaticity and attention in the processing of emotionally significant stimuli. , 2007, Emotion.
[17] Lisa Feldman Barrett,et al. Emotion and Consciousness , 2005 .
[18] J. Eastwood,et al. Negative facial expression captures attention and disrupts performance , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.
[19] A. Ohman,et al. Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[20] J T Todd,et al. Implications of a transient-sustained dichotomy for the measurement of human performance. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[21] Arne Öhman,et al. Automaticity and the Amygdala: Nonconscious Responses to Emotional Faces , 2002 .
[22] E. Fox,et al. Facial Expressions of Emotion: Are Angry Faces Detected More Efficiently? , 2000, Cognition & emotion.
[23] D. Grandjean,et al. Electrophysiological correlates of rapid spatial orienting towards fearful faces. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.
[24] J Tzelgov,et al. Specifying the relations between automaticity and consciousness: a theoretical note. , 1997, Consciousness and cognition.
[25] James F. Juola,et al. Relative Effectiveness of Central, Peripheral, and Abrupt-onset Cues in Visual Attention , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[26] K. Mogg,et al. Orienting of Attention to Threatening Facial Expressions Presented under Conditions of Restricted Awareness , 1999 .
[27] Martin Eimer,et al. Attentional capture by task-irrelevant fearful faces is revealed by the N2pc component , 2007, Biological Psychology.
[28] S. Yantis,et al. Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[29] S. Mineka,et al. Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. , 2001, Psychological review.
[30] M. Gazzaniga,et al. The new cognitive neurosciences , 2000 .
[31] R. Blake,et al. Fearful expressions gain preferential access to awareness during continuous flash suppression. , 2007, Emotion.
[32] S. Yantis,et al. Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[33] L. Pessoa. To what extent are emotional visual stimuli processed without attention and awareness? , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[34] R. Dolan,et al. Effects of Attention and Emotion on Face Processing in the Human Brain An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2001, Neuron.
[35] A. Anderson,et al. Neural Correlates of the Automatic Processing of Threat Facial Signals , 2022 .
[36] Thomas M. Moerland,et al. Fate of unattended fearful faces in the amygdala is determined by both attentional resources and cognitive modulation , 2005, NeuroImage.
[37] S. Hofmann,et al. Electrophysiological correlates of spatial orienting towards angry faces: A source localization study , 2008, Neuropsychologia.
[38] J. Eastwood,et al. Differential attentional guidance by unattended faces expressing positive and negative emotion , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[39] F. McGlone,et al. The role of spatial attention in the processing of facial expression: An ERP study of rapid brain responses to six basic emotions , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[40] M. Eimer,et al. The processing of emotional facial expression is gated by spatial attention: evidence from event-related brain potentials. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[41] A. Öhman,et al. Pavlovian Conditioning to Social Stimuli: Backward Masking and the Dissociation of Implicit and Expl , 1997 .