Effects of isolation-rearing on the development of social behaviors in male Mongolian gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus)

Social isolation has been shown to modify behavioral patterns and neuroendocrine function in many species. In this study, we examined the influence of post-weaning social isolation on the behavioral development and fecal testosterone and corticosterone levels of male Mongolian gerbils. In Experiment 1, isolation-reared (IR) males spent more time engaged in social-sniffing. IR males also showed more aggressive and anxiety-related behaviors than group-reared males (GR; three animals per cage). In Experiment 2, a screen-divided-reared condition (SDR) was examined in addition to GR (two males per cage) and IR males. In the SDR group, two subjects were separated by a wire mesh screen allowing for sensory communication and limited physical contact but not direct social interactions. SDR males showed similar patterns in sniffing and anxiety-related behaviors to IR males; however, an increase in aggression was not observed. No differences in fecal testosterone and corticosterone levels were found among males in different rearing conditions. These results suggest that post-weaning social isolation affects behavioral development without affecting testosterone and corticosterone levels. The behavioral changes observed may therefore be regulated by different mechanisms; increases in social-sniffing and anxiety-related behaviors may be caused by the deprivation of direct social interactions, whereas the enhancement of aggressive behaviors may be caused by the lack of sensory stimulation and limited physical contact with other individuals.

[1]  C. Marsden,et al.  Social isolation increases aggressive behaviour and alters the effects of diazepam in the rat social interaction test , 1996, Behavioural Brain Research.

[2]  J. W. Turner Effects of sustained-release testosterone on marking behavior in the mongolian gerbil , 1979, Physiology & Behavior.

[3]  H. H. Swanson,et al.  Population growth and social structure of confined colonies of mongolian gerbils: Scent gland size and marking behaviour as indices of social status , 1978 .

[4]  R. Elwood,et al.  Paternal and maternal behaviour in the Mongolian gerbil , 1975, Animal Behaviour.

[5]  J. Hagan,et al.  Effects of fluoxetine on social behaviour and plasma corticosteroid levels in female mongolian gerbils , 2003, Behavioural pharmacology.

[6]  G. Trancho,et al.  Stress adaptation and adrenal activity in isolated and crowded rats , 1986, Physiology & Behavior.

[7]  S. Pellis,et al.  Differential rates of attack, defense, and counterattack during the developmental decrease in play fighting by male and female rats. , 1990, Developmental psychobiology.

[8]  M. Morgan,et al.  Incentive motivation and behavioral inhibition in socially-isolated rats , 1975, Physiology & Behavior.

[9]  T. Kikusui,et al.  Scent-marking and sexual activity may reflect social hierarchy among group-living male Mongolian gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus) , 2006, Physiology & Behavior.

[10]  J. Winslow Neuropeptides and non-human primate social deficits associated with pathogenic rearing experience , 2005, International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience.

[11]  T. Kikusui,et al.  Social stress decreases marking behavior independently of testosterone in Mongolian gerbils , 2005, Hormones and Behavior.

[12]  M. Molina-hernandez,et al.  Isolation rearing induced fear-like behavior without affecting learning abilities of Wistar rats , 2001, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.

[13]  S. File,et al.  Diazepam and nicotine increase social interaction in gerbils: a test for anxiolytic action , 2001, Brain Research.

[14]  E. Hull,et al.  Population density and social, territorial, and physiological measures in the gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus). , 1973, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[15]  K. Holloway,et al.  Play deprivation without social isolation: housing controls. , 2004, Developmental psychobiology.

[16]  G. Hole The effects of social deprivation on levels of social play in the laboratory rat Rattus norvegicus , 1991, Behavioural Processes.

[17]  A. P. Humphreys,et al.  Isolation has permanent effects upon the behavior of the rat, but not the mouse, gerbil, or guinea pig. , 1981, Developmental psychobiology.

[18]  R. Palme,et al.  Hormones as indicators of stress. , 2002, Domestic animal endocrinology.

[19]  T. Insel,et al.  Infant's response to social separation reflects adult differences in affiliative behavior: a comparative developmental study in prairie and montane voles. , 1990, Developmental psychobiology.

[20]  T. Kikusui,et al.  Social-defeat stress suppresses scent-marking and social-approach behaviors in male Mongolian gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus) , 2006, Physiology & Behavior.

[21]  S. Pellis,et al.  Play fighting of rats in comparative perspective: a schema for neurobehavioral analyses , 1998, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[22]  M. Sánchez,et al.  Effects of prolonged social isolation on responses of neurons in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, preoptic area, and hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus to stimulation of the medial amygdala , 1995, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[23]  F. Dessì-Fulgheri,et al.  Influence of long-term isolation on the production and metabolism of gonadal sex steroids in male and female rats , 1975, Physiology & Behavior.

[24]  J. Panksepp The ontogeny of play in rats. , 1981, Developmental psychobiology.

[25]  C. Hambly,et al.  Faecal corticosterone concentrations indicate that separately housed male mice are not more stressed than group housed males , 2006, Physiology and Behavior.

[26]  A. Dalhouse,et al.  Social isolation in the gerbil: its effect on exploratory or agonistic behavior and adrenocortical activity. , 1973, Physiology & behavior.

[27]  N. Starkey,et al.  The effects of individual housing on ‘anxious’ behaviour in male and female gerbils , 2007, Physiology & Behavior.

[28]  J. Feldon,et al.  Strain differences in the isolation-induced effects on prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response and on locomotor activity. , 2000, Behavioral neuroscience.

[29]  L. A. Syme Social isolation at weaning: Some effects on two measures of activity , 1973 .

[30]  G. Lindzey,et al.  Androgen Control of Territorial Marking in the Mongolian Gerbil , 1968, Science.

[31]  S. File,et al.  Anxiolytic actions of the substance P (NK1) receptor antagonist L-760735 and the 5-HT1A agonist 8-OH-DPAT in the social interaction test in gerbils , 2001, Brain Research.

[32]  D. Cardinali,et al.  24-hour changes in circulating prolactin, follicle-stimulating hormone, luteinizing hormone and testosterone in male rats subjected to social isolation , 2004, Journal of Circadian Rhythms.

[33]  F. Hall,et al.  Social deprivation of neonatal, adolescent, and adult rats has distinct neurochemical and behavioral consequences. , 1998, Critical reviews in neurobiology.

[34]  W. Zhong,et al.  Ecology and social behaviour of Mongolian gerbils,Meriones unguiculatus, at Xilinhot, Inner Mongolia, China , 1989, Animal Behaviour.

[35]  B. M. Spruijt,et al.  Isolation during the play period in infancy decreases adult social interactions in rats , 1999, Behavioural Brain Research.

[36]  F. S. Hall,et al.  Differential basis of strain and rearing effects on open-field behavior in Fawn Hooded and Wistar rats , 2000, Physiology & Behavior.

[37]  N. Sachser The effects of long-term isolation on physiology and behavior in male guinea pigs , 1986, Physiology & Behavior.

[38]  Zuoxin Wang,et al.  Species differences in anxiety-related responses in male prairie and meadow voles: The effects of social isolation , 2005, Physiology & Behavior.

[39]  J. Feldon,et al.  Effect of social isolation on stress-related behavioural and neuroendocrine state in the rat , 2004, Behavioural Brain Research.

[40]  M. Morgan,et al.  Early Isolation Produces Enduring Hyperactivity in the RAT, But no Effect upon Spontaneous Alternation , 1978, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[41]  B. Probst Individual marking activities not reflected by respective testosterone levels in male gerbils , 1985, Physiology & Behavior.

[42]  S. Pellis,et al.  Mechanisms underlying the absence of the pubertal shift in the playful defense of female rats. , 1998, Developmental psychobiology.

[43]  C. Marsden,et al.  Resocialisation of isolation-reared rats does not alter their anxiogenic profile on the elevated X-maze model of anxiety , 1991, Physiology & Behavior.

[44]  Y. Delville,et al.  Glucocorticoids and the Development of Agonistic Behaviour during Puberty in Male Golden Hamsters , 2005, Journal of neuroendocrinology.

[45]  H. Yoshimura Behavioral characteristics of scent marking behavior in the Mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus). , 1981, Jikken dobutsu. Experimental animals.

[46]  B. Galef,et al.  Why some male Mongolian gerbils may help at the nest: testosterone, asexuality and alloparenting , 2000, Animal Behaviour.

[47]  Linda P Spear,et al.  Social Behavior and Social Motivation in Adolescent Rats: Role of Housing Conditions and Partner's Activity , 1999, Physiology & Behavior.

[48]  S. Blum The effect of different amounts of androgen on scent marking in the male Mongolian gerbil*1 , 1971 .

[49]  M. Morgan,et al.  Incentive motivation and behavioral inhibition in socially-isolated rats. , 1975 .

[50]  C. Marsden,et al.  Influence of Postweaning Social Isolation in the Rat on Brain Development, Conditioned Behavior, and Neurotransmission , 2004, Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology.

[51]  A. Lajtha,et al.  Effects of social isolation and the time of day on testosterone levels in plasma of C57BL/6By and BALB/cBy mice , 1990, Steroids.

[52]  Hall Fs Social Deprivation of Neonatal, Adolescent, and Adult Rats Has Distinct Neurochemical and Behavioral Consequences , 1998 .