Disruptive communication: stealth signaling in the toadfish

Male oyster toadfish, Opsanus tau, produce long duration (250 to 650 ms) sexual advertisement calls or ‘boatwhistles’ during the breeding season. When males are in close proximity, the fishes alternate the production of boatwhistles with other males to avoid call overlap. However, males can also produce a number of different sounds, including a single, short duration pulse or ‘grunt’ (~100 ms). The vocalizations of competing males were recorded in situ with multiple hydrophones to examine intraspecific interactions. These short grunts were emitted almost exclusively during the boatwhistle of a conspecific male. The fundamental frequency (or pulse repetition rate) of the boatwhistles were modified by this disruptive grunt, ‘jamming’ the signal and decreasing its frequency. The disruptive grunt specifically targeted the second stage or tonal portion of the boatwhistle, believed to be the primary acoustic attractant for females, and its brevity and precision may allow its emitter to remain undetectable. While the acoustic repertoire of teleost fishes may be less diverse compared with terrestrial species, the disruptive grunts indicate fish have the capacity for complex acoustic interactions.

[1]  R. Fay,et al.  Dendritic arbors and central projections of physiologically characterized auditory fibers from the saccule of the toadfish, Opsanus tau , 1999, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[2]  A. Bass,et al.  Neural mechanisms and behaviors for acoustic communication in teleost fish , 2003, Progress in Neurobiology.

[3]  R. Fay,et al.  Directional response properties of saccular afferents of the toadfish, Opsanus tau , 1997, Hearing Research.

[4]  L. Rome,et al.  VARIATIONS OF PULSE REPETITION RATE IN BOATWHISTLE SOUNDS FROM OYSTER TOADFISH OPSANUS TAU AROUND WAQUOIT BAY, MASSACHUSETTS , 2002 .

[5]  A. Mensinger,et al.  Acoustic characteristics and variations in grunt vocalizations in the oyster toadfish Opsanus tau , 2009, Environmental Biology of Fishes.

[6]  Michael D Greenfield,et al.  Cooperation and Conflict in the Evolution of Signal Interactions , 1994 .

[7]  Andrew H. Bass,et al.  Alternative male spawning tactics and acoustic signals in the plainfin midshipman fish , 2010 .

[8]  D. Bodznick,et al.  An adaptive filter that cancels self-induced noise in the electrosensory and lateral line mechanosensory systems of fish , 1994, Neuroscience Letters.

[9]  L. Rome,et al.  The whistle and the rattle: the design of sound producing muscles. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[10]  M. Fine,et al.  Use of Passive Acoustics for Assessing Behavioral Interactions in Individual Toadfish , 2008 .

[11]  R. Hanlon,et al.  Mariculture of the Oyster Toadfish: Juvenile Growth and Survival , 2003 .

[12]  J. Fish The Effect of Sound Playback on the Toadfish , 1972 .

[13]  M. Amorim,et al.  Chorusing Behaviour in the Lusitanian Toadfish: Should I Match My Neighbours' Calling Rate? , 2012 .

[14]  M. Fine,et al.  Acoustic competition in the gulf toadfish Opsanus beta: acoustic tagging. , 2002, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[15]  M L Lenhardt,et al.  Shallow-water propagation of the toadfish mating call. , 1983, Comparative biochemistry and physiology. A, Comparative physiology.

[16]  A. Bass,et al.  Vocal Pathways Modulate Efferent Neurons to the Inner Ear and Lateral Line , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  M. Fine,et al.  Seasonal and geographical variation of the mating call of the oyster toadfish Opsanus tau L. , 2004, Oecologia.

[18]  H. E. Winn Acoustic Discrimination by the Toadfish with Comments on Signal Systems , 1972 .

[19]  H. E. Winn,et al.  Reproductive Ecology and Sound Production of the Toadfish, Opsanus Tau , 1961 .

[20]  Friedrich Ladich,et al.  Agonistic behaviour and significance of sounds in vocalizing fish , 1997 .