Measuring gaze overlap on videos between multiple observers

For gaze-based training in surgery to be meaningful, the similarity between a trainee's gaze and an expert's gaze during performance of surgical tasks must be assessed. As it is difficult to record two people's gaze simultaneously, we produced task videos made by experts, and measured the amount of overlap between the gaze path of the expert surgeon and third-party observers while watching the videos. For this investigation, we developed a new, simple method for displaying and summarizing the proportion of time during which two observers' points of gaze on a common stimulus were separated by no more than a specified visual angle. In a study of single-observer self-review and multiple-observer initial view of a laparoscopic training task, we predicted that self-review would produce the highest overlap. We found relatively low overlap between watchers and the task performer; even operators with detailed task knowledge produce low overlap when watching their own videos. Conversely, there was a high overlap among all watchers. Results indicate that it may be insufficient to improve trainees' eye-hand coordination by just watching a video. Gaze training will need to be integrated with other teaching methods to be effective.

[1]  H L Kundel,et al.  Peripheral vision, structured noise and film reader error. , 1973, Radiology.

[2]  Necip Berme,et al.  Eye motion parameters correlate with level of experience in video-assisted surgery: objective testing of three tasks. , 2005, Journal of laparoendoscopic & advanced surgical techniques. Part A.

[3]  M. Stella Atkins,et al.  Saccadic delays on targets while watching videos , 2012, ETRA.

[4]  Guang-Zhong Yang,et al.  The Effect of Depth Perception on Visual-Motor Compensation in Minimal Invasive Surgery , 2006, MIAR.

[5]  Mark R. Wilson,et al.  Gaze training enhances laparoscopic technical skill acquisition and multi-tasking performance: a randomized, controlled study , 2011, Surgical Endoscopy.

[6]  S. Hamstra,et al.  Visual-spatial abilities in surgical training. , 2000, American journal of surgery.

[7]  Michael E. Holmes,et al.  Clutter or content?: how on-screen enhancements affect how TV viewers scan and what they learn , 2006, ETRA.

[8]  Marcus Nyström,et al.  A vector-based, multidimensional scanpath similarity measure , 2010, ETRA.

[9]  J. Meredith,et al.  Videotape review leads to rapid and sustained learning. , 2003, American journal of surgery.

[10]  Eduardo Salas,et al.  Planning, Shared Mental Models, and Coordinated Performance: An Empirical Link Is Established , 1999, Hum. Factors.

[11]  C. Nodine,et al.  Using eye movements to study visual search and to improve tumor detection. , 1987, Radiographics : a review publication of the Radiological Society of North America, Inc.

[12]  M. Stella Atkins,et al.  Eye gaze patterns differentiate novice and experts in a virtual laparoscopic surgery training environment , 2004, ETRA.

[13]  Jia Guo,et al.  How Eye Gaze Feedback Changes Parent-child Joint Attention in Shared Storybook Reading ? An Eye-tracking Intervention Study , 2011 .

[14]  Jia Guo,et al.  How Eye Gaze Feedback Changes Parent-Child Joint Attention in Shared Storybook Reading? , 2013, Eye Gaze in Intelligent User Interfaces.

[15]  C. MacKenzie,et al.  A laboratory study on anticipatory movement in laparoscopic surgery: a behavioral indicator for team collaboration , 2007, Surgical Endoscopy.

[16]  R. Johansson,et al.  Eye–Hand Coordination during Learning of a Novel Visuomotor Task , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  L. Stark,et al.  Spontaneous Eye Movements During Visual Imagery Reflect the Content of the Visual Scene , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.