Why Do Skips Precede Reversals? The Effect of Tessitura on Melodic Structure

In melodies from a wide variety of cultures, a large pitch interval tends to be followed by a change of direction. Although this tendency is often attributed to listeners' expectations, it might arise more simply from constraints on melodic ranginess or tessitura. Skips tend toward the extremes of a melody's tessitura, and from those extremes a melody has little choice but to retreat by changing direction. Statistical analyses of vocal melodies from four different continents are consistent with this simple explanation. The results suggest that, in the sampled repertoires, patterns such as "gap-fill," "registral direction," and "registral return" (L. Meyer, 1956, 1973; E. Narmour, 1990) are mere side effects of constraints on melodic tessitura.

[1]  Paul T. von Hippel,et al.  Redefining Pitch Proximity: Tessitura and Mobility as Constraints on Melodic Intervals , 2000 .

[2]  William Forde Thompson,et al.  Expectancy in Bohemian Folk Song Melodies: Evaluation of Implicative Principles for Implicative and Closural Intervals , 1998 .

[3]  E. Schellenberg,et al.  Simplifying the Implication-Realization Model of Melodic Expectancy , 1997 .

[4]  L. Cuddy,et al.  Expectancies generated by melodic intervals: Perceptual judgments of melodic continuity , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  C. Krumhansl Music Psychology and Music Theory: Problems and Prospects , 1995 .

[6]  E. Narmour The Analysis and Cognition of Melodic Complexity: The Implication-Realization Model , 1992 .

[7]  Eugene Narmour,et al.  The Analysis and Cognition of Basic Melodic Structures: The Implication-Realization Model , 1990 .

[8]  M. Schmuckler Expectation in music: Investigation of melodic and harmonic processes. , 1989 .

[9]  Leonard B. Meyer,et al.  Explaining Music: Essays and Explorations , 1975 .

[10]  A. H. F. Strangways The Music of Hindostan , 1967, The Journal of Asian Studies.

[11]  The Traditional Tunes of the Child Ballads , 1963 .

[12]  Leonard B. Meyer Emotion and Meaning in Music , 1957 .

[13]  Willi Apel,et al.  The Harvard dictionary of music , 1946 .

[14]  Donald T. Campbell,et al.  A Primer on Regression Artifacts , 1999 .

[15]  L. Cuddy,et al.  Expectancies generated by melodic intervals: Evaluation of principles of melodic implication in a melody-completion task , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[16]  Schellenberg Eg Expectancy in melody: tests of the implication-realization model , 1996 .

[17]  Annabel J. Cohen,et al.  Tonality and perception: Musical scales primed by excerpts fromThe Well-Tempered Clavier of J. S. Bach , 1991, Psychological research.

[18]  Suk Won Yi A theory of melodic contour as pitch-time interaction : the linguistic modeling and statistical analysis of vocal melodies in selected 'Lied' collections of Schubert and Schumann , 1990 .

[19]  R. Darlington,et al.  Regression and Linear Models , 1990 .

[20]  William Eastman Lake,et al.  Melodic perception and cognition : the influence of tonality , 1987 .

[21]  James R. Pomerantz,et al.  CHAPTER 1 – Visual Form Perception: An Overview* , 1986 .

[22]  Stefan M. Kostka,et al.  Tonal Harmony: With an Introduction to Twentieth Century Music , 1984 .

[23]  J. Moran,et al.  Sensation and perception , 1980 .

[24]  I. Fischer,et al.  A handbook of modal counterpoint , 1967 .

[25]  E. Prout Counterpoint Strict and Free , 2022 .