Evaluating the role of prefrontal and parietal cortices in memory-guided response with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation

The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) plays an important role in working memory, including the control of memory-guided response. In this study, with 24 subjects, we used high frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) to evaluate the role of the dlPFC in memory-guided response to two different types of spatial working memory tasks: one requiring a recognition decision about a probe stimulus (operationalized with a yes/no button press), another requiring direct recall of the memory stimulus by moving a cursor to the remembered location. In half the trials, randomly distributed, rTMS was applied to the dlPFC and in a separate session, the superior parietal lobule (SPL), a brain area implicated in spatial working memory storage. A 10-Hz (3s, 110% of motor threshold) train of TMS was delivered at the onset of the response period. We found that only dlPFC rTMS significantly affected performance, with rTMS of right dlPFC decreasing accuracy on delayed-recall trials, and rTMS of left and right dlPFC decreasing and enhancing accuracy, respectively, on delayed-recognition trials. These findings confirm that the dlPFC plays an important role in memory-guided response, and suggest that the nature of this role varies depending on the processes required for making a response.

[1]  J. Fuster Prefrontal Cortex , 2018 .

[2]  Clayton E. Curtis,et al.  Persistent neural activity during the maintenance of spatial position in working memory , 2008, NeuroImage.

[3]  B. Postle Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain , 2006, Neuroscience.

[4]  John Duncan,et al.  Frontal lobe involvement in spatial span: Converging studies of normal and impaired function , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  C. B. Cave,et al.  Evidence for two types of spatial representations: hemispheric specialization for categorical and coordinate relations. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  D Le Bihan,et al.  The Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex (dlpfc) Plays a Key Role in Working Memory (wm). yet Its Precise Contribution , 2022 .

[7]  John D E Gabrieli,et al.  The role of the prefrontal cortex in the maintenance of verbal working memory: an event-related FMRI analysis. , 2005, Neuropsychology.

[8]  Tracy R. Henderson,et al.  Simple metric for scaling motor threshold based on scalp-cortex distance: application to studies using transcranial magnetic stimulation. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  Neil G. Muggleton,et al.  New light through old windows: Moving beyond the “virtual lesion” approach to transcranial magnetic stimulation , 2008, NeuroImage.

[10]  James B. Rowe,et al.  Dorsal Prefrontal Cortex: Maintenance in Memory or Attentional Selection? , 2002 .

[11]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Handbook of Neuropsychology , 1991 .

[12]  Goldman-Rakic Ps Cellular and circuit basis of working memory in prefrontal cortex of nonhuman primates. , 1990 .

[13]  A. Shimamura,et al.  Frontal Lobes and Memory , 2002 .

[14]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Functional MRI studies of spatial and nonspatial working memory. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[15]  Y Kamitani,et al.  Effects of single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) on functional brain activity: a combined event-related TMS and evoked potential study , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[16]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Visual Working Memory in Prefrontal Cortex of the Macaque , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  M. Rugg,et al.  Event-related potential studies of associative recognition and recall: electrophysiological evidence for context dependent retrieval processes. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[18]  C. Curtis Prefrontal and parietal contributions to spatial working memory , 2006, Neuroscience.

[19]  M. Mesulam Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events. , 1999, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[20]  René M. Müri,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetry in cortical control of memory-guided saccades. A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  C Caltagirone,et al.  Parieto-frontal interactions in visual-object and visual-spatial working memory: evidence from transcranial magnetic stimulation. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[22]  D. Sheehan,et al.  The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. , 1998, The Journal of clinical psychiatry.

[23]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Lateralization of Prefrontal Activity during Episodic Memory Retrieval: Evidence for the Production-Monitoring Hypothesis , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  Bradley R. Postle,et al.  Distraction-spanning sustained activity during delayed recognition of locations , 2006, NeuroImage.

[25]  Giulio Tononi,et al.  Direct evidence for a prefrontal contribution to the control of proactive interference in verbal working memory , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[26]  Clayton E. Curtis,et al.  Maintenance of Spatial and Motor Codes during Oculomotor Delayed Response Tasks , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  Alexander Münchau,et al.  Magnetic stimulation of human premotor or motor cortex produces interhemispheric facilitation through distinct pathways , 2006, The Journal of physiology.

[28]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  The Influence of Working-Memory Demand and Subject Performance on Prefrontal Cortical Activity , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[29]  Danielle C. Turner,et al.  Association Between Response Inhibition and Working Memory in Adult ADHD: A Link to Right Frontal Cortex Pathology? , 2007, Biological Psychiatry.

[30]  N. Stanhope,et al.  Recall and recognition memory in patients with focal frontal, temporal lobe and diencephalic lesions , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  Edward Awh,et al.  The Role of Spatial Selective Attention in Working Memory for Locations: Evidence from Event-Related Potentials , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  L R Squire,et al.  On the relationship between recall and recognition memory. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[33]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  Neural mechanisms for response selection: comparing selection of responses and items from working memory , 2007, NeuroImage.

[34]  M. Rushworth,et al.  A primer of magnetic stimulation as a tool for neuropsychology. , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  S. Neggers,et al.  Brain areas involved in spatial working memory , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  Bradley R Postle,et al.  The dependence of span and delayed-response performance on prefrontal cortex , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  Alvaro Pascual-Leone,et al.  Exploring paradoxical functional facilitation with TMS. , 2003, Supplements to Clinical neurophysiology.

[38]  Eric H. Schumacher,et al.  Neural Mechanisms for Response Selection: Representation Specific or Modality Independent? , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  David E. J. Linden,et al.  Combining transcranial magnetic stimulation and functional imaging in cognitive brain research: possibilities and limitations , 2003, Brain Research Reviews.

[40]  E. Tulving,et al.  Retroactive inhibition in free recall: Inaccessibility of information available in the memory store , 1971 .

[41]  S. Lehéricy,et al.  Specific cerebral networks for maintenance and response organization within working memory as evidenced by the 'double delay/double response' paradigm. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  M. Hamilton A RATING SCALE FOR DEPRESSION , 1960, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[43]  T. Hergueta,et al.  The mini international neuropsychiatric interview , 1998, European Psychiatry.

[44]  Donald T. Stuss,et al.  Remembering and Knowing in Patients with Frontal Lobe Injuries , 2003, Cortex.

[45]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Prefrontal connections of medial motor areas in the rhesus monkey , 1993, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[46]  G. Tononi,et al.  Evaluating frontal and parietal contributions to spatial working memory with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation , 2008, Brain Research.

[47]  Mark D'Esposito Neurological Foundations of Cognitive Neuroscience , 2002 .

[48]  D. Heeger,et al.  Sustained Activity in Topographic Areas of Human Posterior Parietal Cortex during Memory-Guided Saccades , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  I. Rock,et al.  A study of memory for visual form. , 1959 .

[50]  D. Stuss,et al.  Principles of frontal lobe function , 2002 .

[51]  E. Wassermann Risk and safety of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: report and suggested guidelines from the International Workshop on the Safety of Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation, June 5-7, 1996. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[52]  Ranulfo Romo,et al.  Flexible Control of Mutual Inhibition: A Neural Model of Two-Interval Discrimination , 2005, Science.

[53]  Reinhard Dengler,et al.  Subthreshold prefrontal repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces motor cortex excitability , 2000, Muscle & nerve.

[54]  Alvaro Pascual-Leone,et al.  Impact of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the parietal cortex on metabolic brain activity: a 14C-2DG tracing study in the cat , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[55]  C. Polkey,et al.  Spatial Working Memory and Strategy Formation in Patients with Frontal Lobe Excisions , 1996, Cortex.