Implementation of napping In industry and the problem of sleep inertia

SUMMARY  It seems reasonable to believe that in specific situations napping at the work place would be possible and used if authorized and encouraged. Very short naps could have very positive long‐term effects on biological functions. Training someone to sleep for short periods appears feasible if there is a high motivation to do so. Sleep inertia can be considered as one of the main limiting factors in napping strategy. Sleep inertia depends on different factors such as sleep stage preceding the awakening, temporal placement of the nap, duration of nap and wakefulness preceding it, etc. The effects of sleep inertia might be different depending on the type of task, and a reactivation technique applied immediately after awakening may remove it. Despite the fact that its implementation in industry raises some practical issues, napping can be considered as a possible strategy to increase the vigilance level of night workers.

[1]  D. Dijk,et al.  Circadian and sleep/wake dependent aspects of subjective alertness and cognitive performance , 1992, Journal of sleep research.

[2]  R. Rosa,et al.  Recovery of performance during sleep following sleep deprivation. , 1983, Psychophysiology.

[3]  M. Stones Memory performance after arousal from different sleep stages. , 1977, British journal of psychology.

[4]  T. Åkerstedt,et al.  Sleep on the night shift: 24-hour EEG monitoring of spontaneous sleep/wake behavior. , 1989, Psychophysiology.

[5]  G. Hildebrandt,et al.  Circadian system response to night work in relation to the individual circadian phase position , 1979, International archives of occupational and environmental health.

[6]  M. Bonnet,et al.  Training subjective insomniacs to accurately perceive sleep onset. , 1992, Sleep.

[7]  S Folkard,et al.  Circadian characteristics influencing interindividual differences in tolerance and adjustment to shiftwork. , 1989, Ergonomics.

[8]  F. J. Evans,et al.  Appetitive and replacement naps: EEG and behavior. , 1977, Science.

[9]  S Folkard,et al.  Towards a predictive test of adjustment to shift work. , 1979, Ergonomics.

[10]  Lawrence R. Murphy,et al.  Occupational stress management: A review and appraisal , 1984 .

[11]  K. Matsumoto Effects of nighttime naps on body temperature changes, sleep patterns, and self-evaluation of sleep. , 1981, Journal of human ergology.

[12]  W. B. Webb,et al.  The sleep of ‘morning’ and ‘evening’ types , 1978, Biological Psychology.

[13]  D. Haslam Sleep deprivation and naps , 1985 .

[14]  P Lavie,et al.  Twenty-four-hour structure of sleepiness in morning and evening persons investigated by ultrashort sleep-wake cycle. , 1989, Sleep.

[15]  K. Matsumoto,et al.  Effects of nighttime nap and age on sleep patterns of shift workers. , 1987, Sleep.

[16]  J. Horne,et al.  A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. , 1976, International journal of chronobiology.

[17]  A. Muzet,et al.  Effects of Noise on Sleep Inertia as a Function of Circadian Placement of a One-Hour Nap , 1992, Perceptual and motor skills.

[18]  M. Orne,et al.  Temporal placement of a nap for alertness: contributions of circadian phase and prior wakefulness. , 1987, Sleep.

[19]  David F. Dinges,et al.  Assessing performance upon abrupt awakening from naps during quasi-continuous operations , 1985 .

[20]  R. Bootzin,et al.  Behavioral Treatments for Insomnia1 , 1978 .

[21]  R. T. Wilkinson,et al.  Performance after awakening at different times of night , 1971 .

[22]  S. Campbell,et al.  Evidence for circadian influence on human slow wave sleep during daytime sleep episodes. , 1989, Psychophysiology.

[23]  Thomas J. Balkin,et al.  Relationship between sleep inertia and sleepiness: Cumulative effects of four nights of sleep disruption/restriction on performance following abrupt nocturnal awakening , 1988, Biological Psychology.

[24]  S. Campbell,et al.  Napping behavior during "spontaneous internal desynchronization": sleep remains in synchrony with body temperature. , 1985, Human neurobiology.

[25]  T Akerstedt,et al.  Shift work. Shift-dependent well-being and individual differences. , 1981, Ergonomics.

[26]  J. Waterhouse,et al.  Anchor sleep as a synchronizer of rhythms on abnormal routines. , 1981, International journal of chronobiology.